|
Arkes, H. R. (1996). The psychology of waste. Journal of Behavior Decision Making, 9(3), 213-224. Arkes, H. R., &; Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35(1), 124-140. Baron, R. M., &; Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182. Bazerman, M., Beekun, R., &; Schoorman, F. (1982). Performance evaluation in dynamic context: the impact of a prior commitment to the rate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 873-873. Bellemare, C., Krause, M., Kroger, S., &; Zhang, C. (2005). Myopic loss aversion: information feedback vs. invest. Flexibility. Econ. Lett., 87(3), 319-324. Benartzi, S., &; Thaler. R. H. (1995). Myopic loss-aversion and the equity premium puzzle. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110(1), 73-92. Bhushan, R., Brown, D., &; Mello, A. (1997). Do noise traders ‘create their own space? Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 32(1), 25-45. Bonini, C. P. (1977). Capital investment under uncertainty with abandonment options. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 12(1), 39-54. Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing course of action: toward theoretical progress. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 39-61. Chow, C. W., Harrison, P., Lindquist, T., &; Wu, A. (1997). Escalating commitment to unprofitable projects: replication and cross-cultural extension. Management Accounting Research, 8(3), 347-361. Conlon, D. E. &; Garland, H. (1993). The role of project completion information in resource allocation decisions. Academy of Management Review 36, 402-413. Cronbach, L. J. (1987). Statistical tests for moderator variables: flaws in analyses recently proposed. Psychological Bulletin, 102(3), 414-417. Cunha, Jr, M., &; Caldieraro, F. (2009). Sunk-cost effects on purely behavioral investments. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 105-113. Dawes, R. M. (1988). Rational choice in an uncertain world. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Dholakia, U. M. (2001). A motivational process model of product involvement and consumer risk perception. European Journal of Marketing, 35(11/12), 1340-1362. Fan, D. H. (1992). The functions, assumptions, and limitations for analysis of covariance. Journal of National Taiwan Normal University, 37, 133-163 (In Chinese). Garland, H. (1990). Throwing good money after bad: the effect of sunk costs on the decision to escalate commitment to and ongoing project. J. Appl. Psychol., 75(6), 728-731. Garland, H., &; Newport, S. (1991). Effects of absolute and relative sunk cost on the decision to persist with a course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 48(1), 55-69. Hilton, R. W. (2008). Managerial Accounting: Creating Value in a Dynamic Business Environment, 7e. Taiwan: McGraw-Hill Companies. Hilton, R.W. (2010). Managerial Accounting: Creating Value in a Dynamic Business Environment, 8e. Taiwan: McGraw-Hill College. Hongren, C., Foster , G., &; Datar. S. M. (2006). Cost Accounting: A Managerial Emphasis, 9e. Prentice Hall. Hughes, C. T., &; Gibson, M. L. (1991). Students as surrogates for managers in a decision making environment: an experimental study. Journal of Management Information Systems, 8(2), 153-166. Kahneman, D., &; Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. Kahneman, D., &; Tversky, A. (1984). Choices values and frames. Am. Psycholo., 39(4), 341-350. Keil, M. B., Tan, C. Y., Wei, K., Saarine, T., Tuunaine, V., &; Wassenaar, A. (2000b). A cross-cultural study on escalation of commitment behavior in software projects. MIS Q., 24(2), 299-324. Keil, M., Mann, J., &; Rai, A. (2000a). Why software projects escalate: an empirical analysis and test of four theoretical models. MIS Q., 24(4), 631-663. Keil, M., Mixon, R., Saarinen, T., &; Tuunainen V. (1995). Understanding runaway information technology projects: results from and international research program based on escalation theory. Journal of Management Information Systems, 11(3), 67-87. Keil, M., Truex, D. P., &; Mixon, R. (1995). The effects of sunk cost and project completion on information technology project escalation. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, 42(4), 372-381. Kelly, M. (1997). Do noise traders influence stock prices? Journal of Money, Credit and Banking, 29(3), 351-363. Mann, J. (1996). The Role of Project Escalation in Explaining Runaway Information Systems Development Projects: a Field Study. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Georgia State University, Atlanta: GA. March, J.G., &; Shapira, Z. (1987). Managerial perspectives on risk and risk taking. Management Science, 33(11) , 1404-1418. McAfee, R. P., Mialon, H. M., &; Mialon, S. H. (2010). Do sunk costs matter? Economic Inquiry, 48(2), 323-336. Merchant, K.A., &; Bruns, W. J. (1986). Measurements to cure management myopia. Business Horizons, 29(3), 56-64. Merchant, K. A., &; Manzoni, J. F. (1989). The achievability of budget targets in profit centers: a field study. The Accounting Review, 64(3), 539-558. Sharp, D. J., &; Salter, S. B. (1997). Project escalation and sunk costs: a test of the international generalizability of agency and prospect theory. Journal of International Business Studies, 28(1), 101-121. Shapira, Z. (1995). Risk Taking: A Managerial Perspective. New York: Russel Sage Foundation. Slovic, P. (1969). Differential effects of real versus hypothetical payoffs on choices among gambles. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80(3), 434-437. Snedecor, G. W., &; Cochran, W. G. (1989). Statistical methods. Iowa State University Press: AMES. Soman, D., &; Cheema, A. (2001). The effect of windfall gains on the sunk cost effect. Marketing Letters, 12(1), 51-62. Soman, D., &; Gourville, J. T. (2001). Transaction decoupling: how price bundling affects the decision to consume. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(1), 30-44. Southwood, K. E. (1978). Substantive theory and statistical interactions: five models. American Journal of Sociology, 83(5), 1154-1203. Staw, B. M. (1976). Knee-deep in the big muddy: a study of escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16(1), 27-44. Staw, B. M., &; Ross, J. (1978). Commitment to a policy decision: a muti-theoretical perspective. Administratives Science Quarterly , 23(1), 40-64. Stede, W. A. V. (2000). The relationship between two consequences of budgetary controls: budgetary slack creation and managerial short-term orientation. Accounting, Organization and Society, 25(6), 609-622. Tan, H. T., &; Yates, J. F. (1995). Sunk cost effects: the influences of instruction and future return estimates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63(3), 311-319. Teger, A. I. (1980). Too Much Invested to Quit: The Psychology of the Escalation of Conflict. New York: Pergamon Press. Thaler, R. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Mark. Sci., 4(3), 199-214. Thaler, R. H. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 1(1), 39-60. Thaler, R. H., Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., &; Schwartz, A. (1997). The effect of myopia and loss aversion on risk taking: an experimental test. Q. J. Econ., 112(2), 647-661. Tversky, A., &; Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertainty, 5(4), 297-323. Whyte, G. (1986). Escalating commitment to a course of action: a reinterpretation. Academy of Management Review, 11(2), 311-321. Whyte, G. (1993). Escalating commitment in individual and group decision-making: a prospect theory approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54(3), 430-455. Yang, M. L., Cheng, K. C. &; Ni, F. Y. (2009). The relationship between risk perception and sunk cost effect. Journal of Management &; Systems, 16(4), 487-504(In Chinese).
|