一、中文部份
林寶貴、林美秀(民82)。學前兒童語言障礙評量表。
許洪坤(民73)。中國兒童學習國語及語言發展階段研究。漢學研究通,3卷,四期,219-220。
張欣戊(1991)。學前兒童語言能力測驗。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究計畫成果報告。國立台灣大學心理系。
張顯達(2003)。「學前兒童語言能力的測驗」的使用。國立台灣大學。
張顯達、張鑑如、柯華葳、蔡素娟(2011)。台灣兒童語言語料庫之建置(NSC-96-2420-H-002-030)。
黃茜郁(民105)。華語幼兒在自然對話中以語意為基礎之句法使用分析(未出版之碩士論文)。國立高雄師範大學,高雄市。葉家利(民97)。臺灣華語幼兒名詞和動詞之習得研究(未出版之碩士論文)。私立輔仁大學,新北市。劉惠美、曹峰銘(2010)。華語嬰幼兒溝通發展量表(台灣版)(MCDI-T)。
劉惠美、陳昱君(民104)。華語嬰幼兒表達性詞彙的語意內容及詞類組成之發展。教育心理學報,47卷,2期,217-242。二、英文部份
Akhtar, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in children’s early word learning. Child Development, 67, 635–645.
Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring basic word order: evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structures. Journal of Child Language, 26, 339-356.
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A.L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 239–273.
Arunachalam, S. & Waxman, S.R. (2011). Grammatical form and semantic context in verb learning. Language Learning and Development, 7, 169-184.
Baldwin, D.A. (1991). Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62(5), 875-890.
Brady, T.F., & Oliva, A. (2008). Statistical learning using real-world scenes: Extracting categorical regularities without conscious intent. Psychological Science, 19, 678–685.
Brojde, C.L., Ahmed, S., & Colunga, E. (2012). Bilingual and Monolingual Children Attend to Different Cues When Learning New Words. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 155.
Brown R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brown, J., Acel, B., Jimenez, L. Kaufman S.B. and Grant, K.P. (2010). Intact implicit learning in autism spectrum conditions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 1-24.
Childers, J.B., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional actions from massed or distributed exposures. Developmental psychology, 38(6), 967-978.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Thepry of Syntax. MIT Press.
Christiansen, M.H. & Monaghan, P. (2006). Discovering verbs through multiple-cue integration. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R.M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets words: How children learn verbs (pp. 88-107). New York: Oxford University Press.
Colonnesi, C. Stams, G.J.J.M., Koster, I. & Noom, M.J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30, 352-366.
Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H. (2005). Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 24–39.
Conwell E. & Morgan J.L. (2012). Is It a Noun or Is It a Verb? Resolving the Ambicategoricality Problem. Language Learning and Development, 8(2), 87-112.
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271-283.
Diesendruck, G., Markson, L., Akhtar, N., & Reudor, A. (2004). Two-year-olds’ sensitivity to speakers’ intent: An alternative account of Samuelson and Smith. Developmental Science, 7, 33–41.
D’Odorico, L., & Fasolo, M. (2007). Nouns and verbs in vocabulary acquisition of Italian children. Journal of Child Language, 34(4), 891-907.
Fiser, J., & Aslin, R.N. (2002). Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 458–467.
Fitneva, S.A., Christiansen M.H., & Monaghan P. (2009). From sound to syntax: phonological constraints on children's lexical categorization of new words. Journal of Child Language, 36, 967-997.
Fusaro, M., & Harris, P.L. (2013). Dax Gets the Nod: Toddlers Detect and Use Social Cues to Evaluate Testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 514–522.
Gebhart, A.L., Newport, E.L., & Aslin, R.N. (2009). Statistical learning of adjacent and nonadjacent dependencies among nonlinguistic sounds. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 486–490.
Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. Language, culture and cognition, 3, 215-256.
Gerken LA, Wilson R, & Lewis W. (2005). Infants can use distributional cues to form syntactic categories. Journal of Child Language, 32, 249–268.
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science, 17, 684–691.
Gleitman, L.R. (1990). The structural sources of verb meaning. Language acquisition, 1, 3–55.
Gliga, T., & Csibra, G. (2009). One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychological Science, 20(3), 347-353.
Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Hudry, K., Charman, T., & Johnson, M.H. (2012). Gaze following, gaze reading, and word learning in children at risk for autism. Child Development, 83(3), 926-938.
Goldfield, B.A. & Reznick, J.S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17, 171-183.
Gomez, R.L. & Gerken, L.A. (2000). Artificial grammar learning by one-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109-135.
Goldstein, M.H., King, A.P., & West, M.J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 8030–3035.
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference. Developmental Science, 13(1), 252-263.
Hauser, M.D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W.T. (2002). The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.
Hsu, H.J., & Bishop, D.V.M. (2011). Grammatical Difficulties in Children with Specific Language Impairment: Is Learning Deficient? Human Development, 53, 264-277.
Imai, M., Li, L., Haryu, E,, Okada, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., Shigematsu, J. (2008). Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children. Child Development, 79, 979–1000.
Iverson, J.M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367-371.
Jaswal, V.K., & Hansen, M.B. (2006). Learning words: children disregard some pragmatic information that conflicts with mutual exclusivity. Developmental Science, 9(2), 158-165.
Kelly, M. (1992). Using sound to solve syntactic problems: The role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. Psychological Review, 99, 349–363.
Krogh, L., Vlach, H.A., & Johnson, S.P. (2012). Statistical learning across development: flexible yet constrained. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 598.
Kuhl, P.K., Williams, K.A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K.N. & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255, 606-608.
Kuhl, P.K., Tsao, F.M. & Liu, H.M. (2003). Foreign -language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction in phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 9096-9101.
Lieven, E., Pine, J., & Baldwin, G. (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language, 24, 187-219.
Marcus, G.F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S. & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 293, 77-79.
Matthews D., Lieven E., Theakston A. & Tomasello M. (2005). The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. Cognitive Development, 20, 121-136.
Maye J., Werker J.F., & Gerken L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101-B111.
Mayo, J. & Eigsti, I. (2012). Brief Report: A Comparison of Statistical Learning in School-Aged Children with High Functioning Autism and Typically Developing Peers. Journal of Autism Development Disorder, 42, 2476-2485.
Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350–364.
Mintz, T.H. (2003). Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition, 90(1), 91–117.
Mintz , T.H. (2006). Finding the verbs: distributional cues to categories available to young learners. Action Meets Word: How Children Learn Verbs. New York: Oxford University Press; pp. 31–63.
Monaghan, P., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M.H. (2005). The differential contribution of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorization. Cognition, 96, 143–182.
Morales, M., Mundy, P. & Rojas, J. (2000). Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 373-377.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorder, 20(1), 115-128.
Mundy, P. & Gomes, A. (1998). Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the second year. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(3), 469-482.
Nappa, R., Wessel, A., McEldoon, K.L., Gleitman, L. R., & Trueswell, J.C. (2009). Use of Speaker's Gaze and Syntax in Verb Learning. Language Learning and Development, 5(4), 203-234.
Otto, B. (2010). Language development in early childhood education (3rd edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Paquette-Smith, M. & Johnson, E.K. (2016) Toddlers' use of grammatical and social cues to learn novel words. Language Learning and Development, 12(3), 328-337.
Pelucchi, B., Hay, J.F., & Saffran, J.R. (2009). Statistical Learning in a Natural Language by 8-Month-Old Infants. Child Development, 80, 674-685.
Pinker, S. (1984). Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Pinker, S. (1994). How could a child use verb syntax to learn verb semantics? In: Gleitman LR, Landau B, editors, The acquisition of the lexicon (pp. 377–410). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Redington, M., Chater, N., & Finch, S. (1998). Distributional information: A powerful cue for acquiring syntactic categories. Cognitive Science, 22, 435–469.
Reznick, J.S.; & Goldfield, B.A.(1992). Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology, 28, 406-413.
Romberg, A.R., & Saffran, J.R. (2010). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(6), 906-914.
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Parish-Morris, J., & Golinkoff, R.M. (2009). Live Action: Can Young Children Learn Verbs From Video? Child Development, 80(5), 1360–1375.
Rubino, R.B., & Pine, J.M. (1998). Subject–verb agreement in Brazilian Portuguese: what low error rates hide. Journal of Child Language, 25, 35–39.
Ruble, D.N. & Nakamura, C.Y. (1972). Task orientation versus social orientation in young children and their attention to relevant social cues. Child Development, 43(2), 471-480.
Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N. and Newport, E.L. (1996). Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants. Science, 274, 1926-1928.
Saffran, J.R. (2003). Statistical Language Learning: Mechanisms and Constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110-114.
Saffran, J.R., & Wilson, D.P. (2003). From syllables to syntax: Multi-level statistical learning by 12- month-old infants. Infancy, 4, 273–284.
Schapiro, A. & Turk-Browne, N. (2015). Statistical Learning. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, 3, 501-506.
Shneidman, L.A., Arroyo, M.E., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). What counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language, 40(3), 672-686.
Samuelson, L.K., & Smith, L.B. (1998). Memory and attention make smart word learning: an alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. Child Development, 69, 94–104.
St Clair, M.C., Monaghan, P., & Christiansen, M.H. (2010). Learning grammatical categories from distributional cues: Flexible frames for language acquisition. Cognition, 116, 341- 360.
Tardif, T., Shatz, M., & Naigles, L. (1997). Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: A comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language, 24, 535-565.
Thompson, L.A., & Massaro, D.W. (1994). Children's Integration of Speech and Gestures in Comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 327-354.
Thorpe, K., & Fernald, A. (2006). Knowing what a novel word is not: Two-year-olds “listen through” ambiguous adjectives in fluent speech. Cognition, 100(3), 389–433.
Tomasello, M., (1992). First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639–650.
Tomasello, M. and Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10, 201-224.
Tomasello, M. (2000a). The social-pragmatic theory of word learning. Pragmatics, 10(4), 401–413.
Tomasello, M. (2000b). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 156–163.
Tomasello, M. (2001). Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life. In M. T. E. Bates (Ed.), Language development: The essential readings (pp. 111-128).
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waxman, S., Fu, X., Arunachalam, S., Leddon, E., Geraghty, K., & Song, H. (2013). Are Nouns Learned Before Verbs? Infants Provide Insight into a Longstanding Debate. Child Development Perspectives, 7(3), 10.
Zeeland, A.A.S., McNealy, K., Wang A.T., Sigman M., Bookhelmer, S.Y. & Dapretto, M. (2010). No Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning During Exposure to Artificial Languages in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 345-351.