References
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49-66.
Blevins, J. (1996). The syllable in phonological theory. In J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp.206-244). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Bourassa, D., & Treiman, R. (2003). Spelling in Children with Dyslexia: Analysis from the Treiman-Bourassa early spelling test. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 309-333.
Bowey, J. A., & Hansen, J. (1994). The development of orthographic rimes as units of word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 465-488.
Bowey, J. A., & Underwood, N. (1996). Further evidence that orthographic rime usage in nonword reading increase with word-level reading proficiency. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 526-562.
Bowey, J. A., Vaughan, L., & Hansen, J. (1998). Beginning readers’ use of orthographic analogies in word reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 108-133.
Bowey, J. A. (1999). The limitations of orthographic rime analogies in beginners’ word reading: A reply to Goswami (1999). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 220-231.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421.
Brown, G. D. A., & Deavers, R. P. (1999). Units of analysis in nonword reading: Evidence from children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 208-242.
Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1992). Learning to pronounce words: The limitations of analogies. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 374-388.
Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., MacLean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407-428.
Chen, S. W. (2006). Phonological processing unit transfer: The impact of first language syllable structure and its implications for preferred subsyllabic division units. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland.
Christensen, C. A., & Bowey, J. A. (2005). The efficacy of orthographic rime, grapheme-phoneme correspondence, and implicit phonics approaches to teaching decoding skills. Scientific studies of reading, 9, 327-349.
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 535-555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical acess in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies in information processing (pp. 151-216). London: Academic.
Coltheart, V., & Leahy, J. (1992). Children’s and adults’ reading of nonwords: Effects of regularity and consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 718-729.
Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (1997). How important are rhyme and analogy in the beginning reading? Cognition, 63, 171-208.
Ehri, L. C. (1991). Development of the ability to read words. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 383-417). New York, NY: Longman.
Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P. B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ehri, L. C., & Robbins, C. (1992). Beginners need some decoding skill to read words by analogy. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 12-26.
Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Novy, D. M., & Liberman, D. (1991). How letter-sound instruction mediates progress in first-grade reading and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 456-469.
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301-330). London: Erlbaum.
Fudge, E. C. (1969). Syllables. Journal of Linguistics, 5, 253-286.
Fudge, E. C. (1987). Branching structure within the syllable. Journal of Linguistics, 23, 359-377.
Geudens, A., & Sandra, D. (2003). Beyond implicit phonological knowledge: No support for an onset-rime structure in children’s explicit phonological awareness. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 157-182.
Gillon, G. (2004). Phonological awareness: From research to practice. New York: The Guilford Press.
Glushko, R. J. (1981). Principles for pronouncing print: The psychology of phonography. In A. M. Lesgold & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.), Interactive processes in reading (pp. 61-84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Goswami, U. (1986). Children’s use of analogy in learning to read: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 73-83.
Goswami, U. (1990). Phonological priming and orthographic analogies in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 323-340.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Goswami, U. (1991). Learning about spelling sequences: The role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading. Children Development, 62, 1110-1123.
Goswami, U., & Mead, F. (1992). Onset and rime awareness and analogies in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 152-162.
Goswami, U. (1993). Toward an interactive analogy model of reading development: Decoding vowel graphemes in beginning reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 443-475.
Goswami, U. (1995). Phonological development and reading by analogy: What is analogy, and what is it not? Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 139-145.
Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & de Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19-52
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235-247.
Gottardo, A.,Yan, B., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 530-542.
Hansen, J. G. (2001). Linguistic constraints on the acquisition of English syllable codas by native speakers of Mandarin Chinese. Applied Linguistics, 22, 338-365.
Ho, C. S-H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276-289.
Ho, C. S-H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Development of phonological awareness of Chinese children in Hong Kong. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 109-126.
Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). The role of reading by analogy in first grade Finnish readers. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 46, 83-98.
Hong,Y. L., Huang, S. S., Jhou, Y. L., Liou, Y. M., Lin, J. R., & Sie, L. S. 洪燕玲、黃秀霜、周奕良、柳雅梅、林娟如、謝麗雪 (2006)。英語認字測驗。心理出版社。
Hu, C. F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55-79.
Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 2-28.
Kamhi, A. G., & Laing, S. P. (2000). The role of analogy in learning to read. Log Phon Vocol, 25, 29-34.
Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (1997). Syllable structure and the distribution of phonemes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 295-311.
Kirtley, C., Bryant, P., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L. (1989) Rhyme, rime, and the onset of reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 224-245.
Masonheimer, P. E., Drum, P. A., & Ehri. L. C. (1984). Does environmental print identification lead children into word reading? Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 257-271.
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor S. (1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 370-396.
Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset-rime segmentation, predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 154-167.
Pick, A., Unze, M., Brownell, C. A., Drozdal, J. G., & Hopman, M. K. (1978). Young children’s knowledge of word structure. Child Development, 49, 669-680.
Peterson, M. E., & Haines, L. P. (1992). Orthographic analogy training with kindergarten children: Effects of analogy use, phonemic segmentation, and letter-sound knowledge. Journal of Reading Behavior, 24, 109-127.
Read, C., Zhang, Y., Nie, H., & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic spelling. Cognition, 24, 31-44.
Rickard Liow, S., & Poon, K. (1998). Phonological awareness in multilingual Chinese children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 339-362.
Savage, R. S., Frederickson, N., Goodwin, R., Patni, U., Smith, N., & Tuersley, L. (2005). Relationships among rapid digit naming, phonological processing, motor automaticity, and speech perception in poor, average, and good readers and spellers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 12-28.
Selkirk, E. O. (1982). The syllable. In H. Van der Hulst, & N. Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations (Part II) (pp. 337-383). Dordrecht: Foris.
Share, D. L. (2004). Knowing letter names and learning letter sounds: A causal connection. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 213-233.
Treiman, R. (1985). Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: Evidence from children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 161-181.
Treiman, R. (1986). The division between onsets and rimes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 476-491.
Treiaman, R., Goswami, U., & Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implication for reading development and theory. Memory and Cognition, 18, 559-567.
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (1995). In defense of an onset-rime syllable structure for English. Language and Speech, 38, 127-142.
Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 124, 107-136.
Uhry, J. K., & Ehri, L. C. (1999). Ease of segmenting two- and three-phoneme words in kindergarten: Rime cohesion or vowel salience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 594-603.
Walton, P. D., Walton, L. M., & Felton, K. (2001). Teaching rime analogy or letter recoding strategies to prereaders: Effects on prereading skills and word reading. Journal of Educational psychology, 93, 160-180.
Wang, B.W. 王備五 (2003)。英語的音節結構:理論與教學。英語教學,27(3),1-14頁。Wise, B. W., Olson, R. K., & Treiman, R. (1990). Subsyllabic units in computerized reading instruction: Onset-rime vs. postvowel segmentation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 1-19.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across language: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29