|
Ansorge, U. (2003). Spatial Simon effects and compatibility effects induced by observed gaze direction. Visual Cognition, 10(3), 363-383. Ansorge, U., & Wuhr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon Effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30(2), 365-377. Bae, G. Y., Choi, J. M., Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2009). Transfer of magnitude and spatial mappings to the SNARC Effect for parity Judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 35(6), 1506-1521. Baron-Cohen, S., & Cross, P. (1992). Reading the eyes: Evidence for the role of perception in the development of a theory of mind. Mind & Language, 7(1-2), 172-186. Danziger, S., Kingstone, A., & Ward, R. (2001). Environmentally defined frames of reference: Their time course and sensitivity to spatial cues and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 494-503. De Houwer, J. (2004). Spatial Simon effects with nonspatial responses. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 49-53. De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20(4), 731-750. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396. Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. (2004). Mechanisms of eye gaze perception during infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1320-1326. Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2(1), 95-110. Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(3), 415-423. Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44(4), 386-392. Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659-673. Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., & Fias, W. (2006). Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 58-68. Gibson, B. S., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Visual Attention and the Semantics of Space: Beyond Central and Peripheral Cues. Psychological Science, 17(7), 622-627. Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondence on two-choice response-time. Acta Psychologica, 39(6), 427-439. Hiroaki, M., Noriyoshi, T., & Katuo, Y. (2002). An electrophysiological study of the locus of the interference effect in a stimulus-response compatibility paradigm: Errata. Psychophysiology, 39(5), 684. Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 56(4), 261-268. Hung, Y.-h., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J., & Wu, D. H. (2008). Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers. Cognition, 106(3), 1441-1450. Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32, 662-673. Klapp, S. T., Greim, D. M., Mendicino, C. M., & Koenig, S. (1979). Anatomic and environmental dimensions of stimulus-response compatibility: Implication for theories of memory coding. Acta Psychologica, 43(5), 367-379. Lammertyn, J., Fias, W., & Lauwereyns, J. (2002). Semantic influences on feature-based attention due to overlap of neural circuits. Cortex, 38(5), 878-882. Lorch, R. F., Jr., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16(1), 149-157. Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(2), 174-207. Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006). Shared spatial representations for numbers and space: the reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32(5), 1197-1207. Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: A Time-Course analysis. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 219-227. Proctor, R. W., & Lu, C.-H. (1999). Processing irrelevant location information: Practice and transfer effects in choice-reaction tasks. Memory & Cognition, 27(1), 63-77. Proctor, R. W., Marble, J. G., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2000). Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: Effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect. Psychological Research, 64, 11-24. Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 446-461. Ricciardelli, P., Bonfiglioli, C., Iani, C., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2007). Spatial Coding and Central Patterns: Is There Something Special About the Eyes? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 79-90. Rubichi, S., & Pellicano, A. (2004). Does the Simon effect affect movement execution? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(6), 825-840. Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Reading space into numbers--A cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect. Cognition, 108(2), 590-599. Simon, J. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174-176. Simon, J. R. (1990). The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional Cue on Human Information Processing. Oxford, England: North-Holland. Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., Umilta, C., & Bassignani, F. (2000). The Role of Long-Term-Memory and Short-Term-Memory Links in the Simon Effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26(2), 648-670. Valle-Inclan, F., & Redondo, M. (1998). On the automaticity of ipsilateral response activation in the Simon effect. Psychophysiology, 35(4), 366-371. Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27(3), 731-751. Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of Stimulus-Response correspondence: Evidence for two Mechanisms involved in the Simon Effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31(3), 453-464. Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Crossed hands and the SNARC effect: A failure to replicate Denhaene, Bossini and Giraux (1993). Cortex, 42(8), 1060-1079. Zorzi, M., Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umilta, C. (2003). Automatic spatial coding of perceived gaze direction is revealed by the Simon effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 423-429. Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umilta, C. (2002). Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417(6885), 138-139.
|