|
Abrams, M.H (2005). A glossary of literary terms /(8th ed.). Boston: Thomson, Wadsworth. Alain, C., & Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, D202-212. Barnea, A., & Breznitz, Z. (1998). Phonological and Orthographic Processing of Hebrew Words: Electrophysiological Aspects. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159(4), 492-504. Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M. H., Echallier, J. F., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(3), 235-260. Booth, J. R., Cho, S., Burman, D. D., & Bitan, T. (2007). Neural correlates of mapping from phonology to orthography in children performing an auditory spelling task. Developmental Science, 10(4), 441-451. Cao, F., Khalid, K., Lee, R., Brennan, C., Yang, Y., Li, K., et al. (2011). Development of brain networks involved in spoken word processing of Mandarin Chinese. NeuroImage, 57(3), 750-759. Caplan, D. (1993). Language: Structure, processing, and disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chen, X., & Kao, H. S. R.. (2002). Visual-spatial properties and orthographic processing of Chinese characters. In H. S. R., Kao, C. K. Leong, & D-G. Gao (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience Studies of the Chinese language (pp. 176–194). Hong Kong: University of Press. Coch, D., Sanders, L. D., & Neville, H. J. (2005). An event-related potential study of selective auditory attention in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(4), 605-622. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204-256. Connolly, J. F., & Phillips, N. A. (1994). Event-related potential components reflect phonological and semantic processing of the terminal word of spoken sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(3), 256-266. Connolly, J. F., Service, E., D'Arcy, R. C. N., Kujala, A., & Alho, K. (2001). Phonological aspects of word recognition as revealed by high-resolution spatio-temporal brain mapping. NeuroReport, 12(2), 237-243. Coxhead, P. (2012, May 1). Re: A glossary of linaueatic terms [Online forum comment]. Retrieved from http: // www. cs.bham.ac.uk /~ pxc/nlp/nlpgloss.html#g-entries Crowley, K. E., & Colrain, I. M. (2004). A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: Age, sleep and modality. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(4), 732-744. D'Arcy, R. C. N., Connolly, J. F., & Crocker, S. F. (2000). Latency shifts in the N2b component track phonological deviations in spoken words. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(1), 40-44. Dehaene, S., Le Clec'H, G., Poline, J. B., Le Bihan, D., & Cohen, L. (2002). The visual word form area: A prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus. NeuroReport, 13(3), 321-325. Desroches, A. S., Newman, R. L., & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: Electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1893-1906. Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (2000). ERP effects of listening to speech: Semantic ERP effects. Neuropsychologia, 38(11), 1518-1530. Han, Z., & Bi, Y. (2009). Oral spelling and writing in a logographic language: Insights from a Chinese dysgraphic individual. Brain and Language, 110(1), 23-28. Han, Z., Zhang, Y., Shu, H., & Bi, Y. (2007). The orthographic buffer in writing Chinese characters: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(4), 431-450. Henry, M., Beeson, P., Stark, A., & Rapcsak, S. (2007). The role of left perisylvian cortical regions in spelling☆. Brain and Language, 100(1), 44-52. Hillis, A. E. (Ed.). (2002). The Handbook of Adult Language Disorders:Integrating Conitive Neuropsychology, Neurology, and Rehabiliation. NY: Psychology Press Ltd. Hillis, A. E. (2008). Cognitive processes underlying reading and writing and their neural substrates. Neuropsychology and behavioral neurology, 88, 311-322. Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1991). Natural speech processing: An analysis using event-related brain potentials. Psychobiology, 19(4), 286-300. Huang, K. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of grapheme-phoneme conversion. Neuroscience Letters, 366(3), 254-258. Klingebiel K., & Weekes B. S. 2008. Developmental Dyslexia in Chinese: Behavioral, Genetic and Neuropsychological Issues. In S. P. Law, B. Weeks & A. Wong (Eds.), Language Disorders in Speakers of Chinese (pp.138-168). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Kramer, A. F., & Donchin, E. (1987). Brain Potentials as Indices of Orthographic and Phonological Interaction During Word Matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(1), 76-86. Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C. K. (1994). Psycholinguistics electrified:Event-related brain potential investigations. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 83–143). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc. Law, S.-P., & Or, B. (2001). A Case Study of Acquired Dyslexia and Dysgraphia in Cantonese: Evidence for Nonsemantic Pathways for Reading and Writing Chinese. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(8), 729-748. Law, S.-P., Yeung, O., Wong, W., & Chiu, K. (2005). Processing of semantic radicals in writing Chinese characters: Data from a Chinese dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(7), 885-903. Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chan, W. H., Hsu, C. H., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An event-related potentials study. NeuroReport, 18(2), 147-151. Lin, S. E., Chen, H. C., Zhao, J., Li, S., He, S., & Weng, X. C. (2011). Left-lateralized N170 response to unpronounceable pseudo but not false Chinese characters-the key role of orthography. Neuroscience, 190, 200-206. Liu, C., Zhang, W. T., Tang, Y. Y., Mai, X. Q., Chen, H. C., Tardif, T., et al. (2008). The Visual Word Form Area: Evidence from an fMRI study of implicit processing of Chinese characters. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1350-1361. Liu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2003). ERP Evidence for the Time Course of Graphic, Phonological, and Semantic Information in Chinese Meaning and Pronunciation Decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1231-1247. Liu, Y., Shu, H., & Wei, J. (2006). Spoken word recognition in context: Evidence from Chinese ERP analyses. Brain and Language, 96(1), 37-48. Lu, Q., Tang, Y. Y., Zhou, L., & Yu, Q. (2011). The different time courses of reading different levels of Chinese characters: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 498(3), 194-198. Mado Proverbio, A. (2003). Time course of brain activation during graphemic/phonologic processing in reading: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 87(3), 412-420. Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C. K. (1994). Psycholinguistics electrified:Event-related brain potential investigations. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 83–143). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc. Martin, B.A., Tremblay, K.L., & Stapells, D.R. (2007). Chapter 23. Principles and Applications of Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials. In J. Eggermont, M. Don, & R. Burkard (Eds.), Auditory evoked potentials. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins. Maurer, U., Brandeis, D., & McCandliss, B. D. (2005). Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 1. Michie, P. T., Bearpark, H. M., Crawford, J. M., & Glue, L. C. T. (1990). The nature of selective attention effects on auditory event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 30(3), 219-250. Michie, P. T., Solowij, N., Crawford, J. M., & Glue, L. C. (1993). The effects of between-source discriminability on attended and unattended auditory ERPs. Psychophysiology, 30(2), 205-220. Näätänen, R., & Michie, P. T. (1979). Early selective-attention effects on the evoked potential: A critical review and reinterpretation. Biological Psychology, 8(2), 81-136. Näätänen, R., & Picton, T. (1987). The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology, 24(4), 375-425. Nadeau, S. E., Rothi, L. J. G., & Crosson, B. (Eds.). (2000). Aphasia and Language: Theory to Practice. NY: Guilford Publications,Inc. Nakamura, K., Honda, M., Hirano, S., Oga, T., Sawamoto, N., Hanakawa, T., et al. (2002). Modulation of the visual word retrieval system in writing: A functional MRI study on the Japanese orthographies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(1), 104-115. Newman, R. L., & Connolly, J. F. (2009). Electrophysiological markers of pre-lexical speech processing: Evidence for bottom-up and top-down effects on spoken word processing. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 114-121. Newman, R. L., Connolly, J. F., Service, E., & McIvor, K. (2003). Influence of phonological expectations during a phoneme deletion task: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 40(4), 640-647. Patel, S. H., & Azzam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the Event-Related Potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 2(4), 147-154. Pattamadilok, C., Perre, L., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2009). On-line orthographic influences on spoken language in a semantic task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 169-179. Perre, L., Midgley, K., & Ziegler, J. C. (2009). When beef primes reef more than leaf: Orthographic information affects phonological priming in spoken word recognition. Psychophysiology, 46(4), 739-746. Perre, L., Pattamadilok, C., Montant, M., & Ziegler, J. C. (2009). Orthographic effects in spoken language: On-line activation or phonological restructuring? Brain Research, 1275(C), 73-80. Philipose, L. E., Gottesman, R. F., Newhart, M., Kleinman, J. T., Herskovits, E. H., Pawlak, M. A., et al. (2007). Neural regions essential for reading and spelling of words and pseudowords. Annals of Neurology, 62(5), 481-492. Portney, Leslie Gross, and Watkins, Mary P. (2009). Foundations of clinical research(3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson/Prentice Hall. Purcell, J. J., Napoliello, E. M., & Eden, G. F. (2011). A combined fMRI study of typed spelling and reading. NeuroImage, 55(2), 750-762. Qi, G. Q., Li, X., Yan, T., Wu, J., & Guo, Q. (2010). Different occipito-temporal activations in Chinese literate and illiterate subjects during Chinese character processing. Rapcsak, S. Z., & Beeson, P. M. (2000) Agraphia. In S. E., Nadeau, L. J. G., Rothi, & B. Crosson (EDs.), Aphaisa and Language : Theory to practice (pp. 109-131). New York: The Guilford Press. Rapp, B., & Caramazza, A. (1997). From Graphemes to Abstract Letter Shapes: Levels of Representation in Written Spelling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(4), 1130-1152. Raymer, M. A., & Rothi, L. J. G. (2000) The semantic system. In S. E., Nadeau, L. J. G., Rothi, & B. Crosson (EDs.), Aphaisa and Language : Theory to practice (pp. 109-131). New York: The Guilford Press. Roeltgen, D. P. (2003). Agraphia. In K. M. Knneth & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 126-145). New York: Oxford University Press. Sakurai, Y., Mimura, I., & Mannen, T. (2008). Agraphia for kanji resulting from a left posterior middle temporal gyrus lesion. Behavioural Neurology, 19(3), 93-106. Sakurai, Y., Matsumura, K., Iwatsubo, T., & Momose, T. (1997). Frontal pure agraphia for kanji or kana: Dissociation between morphology and phonology. Neurology, 49(4), 946-952. Schmalzl, L., & Nickels, L. (2006). Treatment of irregular word spelling in acquired dysgraphia: Selective benefit from visual mnemonics. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 16(1), 1-37. Simon, G., Bernard, C., Lalonde, R., & Rebai, M. (2006). Orthographic transparency and grapheme–phoneme conversion: An ERP study in Arabic and French readers. Brain Research, 1104(1), 141-152. Simon, G., Petit, L., Bernard, C., & Rebaï, M. (2007). N170 ERPs could represent a logographic processing strategy in visual word recognition. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3. Sugihara, G., Kaminaga, T., & Sugishita, M. (2006). Interindividual uniformity and variety of the “Writing center”: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 32(4), 1837-1849. Van Den Brink, D., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (2001). Electrophysiological evidence for early contextual influences during spoken-word recognition: N200 versus N400 effects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 967-985. Wambacq, I. J. A., & Jerger, J. F. (2004). Processing of affective prosody and lexical-semantics in spoken utterances as differentiated by event-related potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(3), 427-437. Weekes, B., Yin, W., Su, I., & Chen, M. (2006). The cognitive neuropsychology of reading and writing in Chinese. Language and linguistics, 7(3), 595-617. Weekes, B. S., & Robinson, G. (1997). Semantic anomia without surface dyslexia. Aphasiology, 11(8), 813-825. Woods, D. L. (1995). The component structure of the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked potential. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement, 44, 102-109. Zhang, Q., Zhang, J. X., & Kong, L. (2009). An ERP study on the time course of phonological and semantic activation in Chinese word recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73(3), 235-245. Zhao, J., Guo, J., Zhou, F., & Shu, H. (2011). Time course of Chinese monosyllabic spoken word recognition: Evidence from ERP analyses. Neuropsychologia.
|