跳到主要內容

臺灣博碩士論文加值系統

(18.97.9.171) 您好!臺灣時間:2024/12/10 13:13
字體大小: 字級放大   字級縮小   預設字形  
回查詢結果 :::

詳目顯示

我願授權國圖
: 
twitterline
研究生:李冠樺
研究生(外文):Kuan Hua Li
論文名稱:不同慣用手孩童於辨識與書寫中文字之行為及事件相關電位研究
論文名稱(外文):The Effect of Handedness on Chinese Character Recognition and Production in Children: A Behavioral and ERP Study
指導教授:孟令夫孟令夫引用關係
指導教授(外文):L. F. Meng
學位類別:碩士
校院名稱:長庚大學
系所名稱:職能治療學系
學門:醫藥衛生學門
學類:復健醫學學類
論文種類:學術論文
論文出版年:2013
畢業學年度:101
論文頁數:152
中文關鍵詞:慣用手轉換慣用手寫字鏡像字事件相關電位
外文關鍵詞:handednessconverted left-handerswritingmirrored charactermirror writingERP(CNVN170P300 &; N300–N400 complex)
相關次數:
  • 被引用被引用:2
  • 點閱點閱:665
  • 評分評分:
  • 下載下載:0
  • 收藏至我的研究室書目清單書目收藏:0
背景與目的:研究指出部分孩童有書寫鏡像字的現象,而其中慣用左手孩童書寫鏡像字比例高於慣用右手者,且有些孩童因轉換慣用手後亦出現寫鏡像字的情形。鏡像字的機制目前仍具有爭議,雖有三大假說(運動假說、空間方位假說、視覺優勢假說)欲探討鏡像字的形成機制,但其與慣用手的關係仍不清楚;且寫字認知模型並未提及鏡像字確切的路徑與歷程,更遑論其與慣用手的關係。因此,鏡像字是否與慣用手相關至今仍未定論。本研究藉由事件相關電位來探討不同慣用手孩童執行寫字與識字的伴隨性負電位變化(CNV),與其辨識正像字及鏡像字時各相關波形成份的異同,來了解不同慣用手孩童在寫字與識字的行為及大腦特性。
方法:參與者為(1)未轉換之慣用左手、(2)慣用右手與(3)轉換慣用手孩童等三組,每組各14位(男女各7位,平均年齡8.09歲),執行「聽寫中文字」與「辨識正像字及鏡像字」兩種任務。以無母數克-瓦二氏檢定與魏克森符號等級檢定統計三組間行為表現的異同,並分析事件相關電位資料中兩種任務的伴隨性負電位變化(CNV)、正像字及鏡像字出現後組內的N170、P300及N300–N400 complex波形成份的平均振幅、波峰振幅及潛伏時間等,以了解不同慣用手孩童於各波型及電極間的差異。
結果:在三組有良好的組間控制下,可發現(1)三組間執行寫字與識字兩種任務的行為資料均無達顯著差異;而事件相關電位資料中,(2)寫字任務的CNV於三組間各電極均無達顯著差異;(3)識字任務的CNV僅P8電極在三組間達顯著差異,且三組間之CNV (T8-T7)、N170 (CP2-CP1)、P300 (CP2-CP1、CP6-CP5、P8-P7)、N300–N400 complex (FC2-FC1),均呈現不同的大腦側化性。另外,(4)三組內不同慣用手孩童辨識鏡像字之N170均較正像字小,且鏡像字之P300及N300–N400 complex均較正像字大;慣用右手組的N170於枕葉區(O1、O2、Oz),P300於O1和P7電極,N300–N400 complex於T8電極達顯著差異;慣用左手組的N170於CP6,P300於枕葉區(O1、O2、Oz)和CP6達顯著差異;轉換慣用手組的N170於O1,P300於O2和CP6,N300–N400 complex於額葉中央區(F4、F8、Fz、FC1、FC2、FC5、FC6、T7、T8)達顯著差異。
討論與結論:不同慣用手組在寫字任務與識字任務的伴隨性負電位變化於三組間的差異均不明顯,推論可能是孩童的大腦專擅化仍未發展成熟;而比較三組間對稱的左右電極,可發現不同慣用手組仍具有不同的側化性。另外,在識字任務中,鏡像字的文字意涵較正像字不明顯,又需較大的認知負荷及來處理,且鏡像字有較大的語意整合衝突的情形。慣用右手組在早期的視覺處理階段,枕葉區即能辨識正像字與鏡像字,而在頂葉區需要多一些認知負荷辨識鏡像字之空間屬性;慣用左手組則於中央頂葉區先處理視覺空間屬性,枕葉區需要多一些認知負荷才可辨識兩種字;轉換慣用手組在早期的視覺處理較不明顯,大部分是在晚期階段額葉中央區才處理鏡像字。融合視覺優勢假說與區辨鏡像字的大腦區域,可推測相較於慣用右手孩童,慣用左手與轉換慣用手孩童可能為雙側大腦彼此溝通較強烈,較難建立單側大腦的專擅化,因此可能容易產生鏡像字。
Although mirror writing has long been observed in converted left-handers and other children in primary schools, its underlying mechanism remains unclear. Current neuropsychological models do not account for mirror writing, and the relationship between handedness and mirror writing is still controversial. In this study, we used the ERP technique to explore writing preparation and mirrored character recognition in children with different handedness.
Behavioral (writing performance, accuracy, and reaction time) and ERP data (CNV, N170, P300, and N300–N400 complex) were collected from 14 converted left-handers and 28 consistent left-/right-handers to analyze and explore differences among handedness groups.
We found no significant differences for the behavioral data among three groups in both tasks. ERP data further showed no statistically significant differences in writing task among the three groups for the late CNV at all electrodes. A significant difference in recognizing task was found only at the P8 electrode, and reversed lateralization was noted (CP2-CP1 in the N170 component; CP2-CP1, CP6-CP5, and P8-P7 in the P300 component). In addition, all groups had larger N170 amplitudes, less P300 amplitudes, and less N300–N400 complex amplitudes for normal characters than for mirrored characters. In right-handers, there were significant differences between both characters for N170 (O1, O2, and Oz), P300 (O1 and P7), and N300–N400 complex (T8). In left-handers, there were significant differences between both characters for N170 (CP6) and P300 (O1, O2, and Oz). In converted left-handers, there were significant differences between both characters for N170 (O1), P300 (O2 and CP6), and N300–N400 complex (F4, F8, Fz, FC1, FC2, FC5, FC6, T7, and T8).
Less significant differences among the three groups for the late CNV were observed because language experience and development are not yet fully specialized in children. However, the different handedness groups showed reversed lateralization for characters. Additionally, significantly larger N170 amplitudes are elicited by normal character recognition in children, more cognitive effort for P300 is needed to process mirrored characters, and stronger N300–N400 complex amplitudes are evoked by mirrored characters. Furthermore, right-handed children distinguish between characters in the early visual processing of the occipital areas (N170) and exert more cognitive effort at P7 (P300), whereas left-handed children distinguish between characters at CP6 (N170) and exert more cognitive effort in the occipital areas (P300). Converted left-handers are particularly strong in the late language processing of the frontal areas, particularly at the right hemisphere (N300–N400 complex). Left- and converted left-handers perhaps have more intense inter-hemispheric communication and cannot specialize one hemisphere, so they produce mirror writing more frequently than right-handers do.
Table of Contents
論文指導教授推薦書
論文口試委員審定書
長庚大學授權書-------------------iii
致謝-------------------iv
中文摘要-------------------vi
Abstract-------------------viii
Table of Contents-------------------x
List of Figures-------------------xv
List of Table-------------------xvii
Introduction-------------------1
Research Background-------------------1
Research Purpose-------------------2
Definitions of Terms-------------------3
Literature Review-------------------5
Characteristics of Writing in Different Handedness Groups----------5
Writing posture-------------------5
Writing direction-------------------6
Brain lateralization of writing-------------------6
Mirror writing studies-------------------8
Mirror Writing Mechanism and Related Studies----------------10
Mirror writing-------------------10
Motor hypothesis-------------------11
Spatial orientation hypothesis-------------------12
Visual dominance or visual word-form hypothesis------------12
Writing Mechanisms and Neuropsychological Models-----------------13
Event-related Potential when Writing and Recognizing Character------20
Contingent negative variation(CNV)-------------------20
N170-------------------22
P300-------------------23
N300–N400 complex-------------------23
Summary-------------------24
Research Questions and Hypotheses-------------------25
Method-------------------27
Research Framework and Design-------------------27
Research framework-------------------27
Definition of variables-------------------28
Research method-------------------30
Subjects-------------------30
Population-------------------30
Criteria-------------------30
Sample size planning-------------------31
Materials and Apparatus-------------------32
Assessment for screening-------------------32
Control variables of characters in tasks-------------------36
Electrophysiological recording and capturing-------------------37
Experimental Procedures and Conditions-------------------38
Experimental procedure-------------------38
Experimental condition-------------------40
Tasks and Procedures-------------------40
Experiment 1: writing tasks-------------------40
Experiment 2: recognizing tasks-------------------42
Results-------------------45
Demographic Characteristics-------------------45
Behavioral Data-------------------45
Independent variables-------------------45
Control variables-------------------47
Dependent variables-------------------47
ERP Data-------------------50
Experiment 1: writing task-------------------50
Experiment 2: recognizing task-------------------52
Discussion-------------------63
Subject Selection and Control Variables-------------------63
Brain Electrophysiology when Recognizing and Writing Characters-------64
CNV-------------------64
N170-------------------65
P300-------------------66
N300–N400 complex-------------------67
Behavioral Performance of Different Handedness Groups--------------68
Accuracy and reaction time--------------68
Chinese character writing performance-------------------69
Brain Lateralization of Character Writing and Recognition-------------71
Limitations of the Study-------------------73
Recommendations for Future Research-------------74
Conclusion-------------------75
References-------------------76
Appendixes-------------------88
Appendix 1 The survey questionnaires-------------------88
Appendix 2 The Edinburgh handedness inventory-------------------92
Appendix 3 The hand preference questionnaires-------------------92
Appendix 4 Assessment of eye preference-------------------93
Appendix 5 The basic information questionnaires-------------------94
Appendix 6 Assessment of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III):
verbal intelligence quotation-------------------98
Appendix 7 Assessment of Dynamic Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment
for Children (DOTCA-Ch): spatial perception test-------------------98
Appendix 8 Assessment of Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT): copy and immediate
recall tests-------------------99
Appendix 9 Assessment of Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-
Motor Integration (VMI): motor coordination-------------------99
Appendix 10 The 200 simple characters literacy scale------------100
Appendix 11 The characteristics of characters-------------------101
Appendix 12 Institutional review board-------------------102
Appendix 13 Informed consent document-------------------104
Appendix 14 The demographic characteristics and behavioral data
(right- handed children)-------------------106
Appendix 15 The demographic characteristics and behavioral data
(left-handed children)-------------------107
Appendix 16 The demographic characteristics and behavioral data
(converted left-handed children)-------------------108
Appendix 17 ERP Data: recognizing tasks (Among groups)-------------------109
Appendix 18 ERP Data: recognizing tasks (Within groups)-------------------123
Appendix 19 The proof of training for WISC-------------------131
Appendix 20 Result- parametric method-------------------132

List of Figures
Figure 1 Writing posture-------------------5
Figure 2 Drawing a circle with both hands-------------------6
Figure 3 Positron emission tomography study on writing-------------------7
Figure 4 Cognitive model of spelling and writing-------------------15
Figure 5 Neuropsychological model of writing-------------------17
Figure 6 Cognitive model of linguistic processing component-------------------19
Figure 7 Research framework-------------------27
Figure 8 The distribution of electrodes-------------------38
Figure 9 Experimental procedure-------------------39
Figure 10 Design and procedure of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2-----------44
Figure 11 The head view of grand average of late CNV in right- (blue line),
left- (red line), and converted left-handers (green line)
in writing task---------52
Figure 12 The head view of grand average of late CNV in right- (blue line),
left- (red line), and converted left-handers (green line)
in recognizing task----------54
Figure 13 The head view of grand average of ERP in normal (blue line),
and mirrored characters (red line) for right-handed children--------61
Figure 14 The head view of grand average of ERP in normal (blue line),
and mirrored characters (red line) for left-handed children---------62
Figure 15 The head view of grand average of ERP in normal (blue line),
and mirrored characters (red line) for converted left-handed
children-------------------62
Figure 16 Laterality quotient of handedness groups-------------------63
Figure 17 Subdivisions in callosal area by Witelson’s method-------------------65

List of Tables
Table 1 Central spelling routes of spelling and writing-------------------15
Table 2 Definition of variables-------------------27
Table 3 Included criteria-------------------31
Table 4 Sample size suggested in the literatures-------------------32
Table 5 The characters in tasks-------------------37
Table 6 Demographic characteristics and independent variables
among three groups-------------------46
Table 7 The control variables among three groups-------------------47
Table 8 The dependent variables among three groups-------------------49
Table 9 Wilcoxon signed ranks test (p value) in recognizing task---------50
Table 10 Kruskal-Wallis test for the mean amplitudes of the late CNV
among different handedness groups in writing task-----------------51
Table 11 Kruskal-Wallis test for the mean amplitudes of the late CNV at channels
difference among different handedness groups in writing task--------51
Table 12 Kruskal-Wallis test for the mean amplitudes of the late CNV among
different handedness groups in recognizing task-------------53
Table 13 Kruskal-Wallis test for the mean amplitudes of the late CNV at
channels difference in different handedness groups---------------53
Table 14 Kruskal-Wallis test for mean amplitude, peak amplitude and latency of
N170, P300, and N300–N400 complex in different handedness groups----56
Table 15 Kruskal-Wallis test for mean amplitude, peak amplitude and latency of
N170, P300, and N300–N400 complex at channels difference in
different handedness groups----------56
Table 16 Wilcoxon signed ranks test for the amplitudes (μV) and latency (ms) in the mirrored and normal characters among three groups-------------------60
References
孟令夫(民89)。腦性麻痺與典型兒童手側化發展研究。國立臺灣師範大學特殊教育研究所博士論文,台北市。

徐宛伶(民99)。轉換慣用手孩童執行redundant task之大腦活化特質與兩半球溝通表現。長庚大學臨床行為科學研
究所碩士論文,桃園縣。

楊惠蘭(民96)。兒歌結合部首識字教學對弱勢幼兒識字能力影響之研究。國立台東大學幼兒教育學系碩士論文,台東縣。

楊逸群(民98)。轉換慣用手者腦電生理特性之探討。長庚大學臨床行為科學研究所碩士論文,桃園縣。

陳美文(民92)。國小讀寫困難學生認知能力之分析研究。國立臺灣師範大學特殊教育研究所碩士論文,臺北市。

陳詩涵、張雅晴、孟令夫、李冠樺(2011年12月)。左撇子兒童容易寫鏡像字嗎?臺灣職能治療學會第30次學術研討會。高雄:高雄醫學大學。

陳榮華(民86)。魏氏兒童智力量表(第三版)。台北市:中國行為科學社。

羅于惠、孟令夫(民98)。鏡像字辨認對N170與P300的效應。長庚大學。未出版。

Angrilli, A., Dobel, C., Rockstroh, B., Stegagno, L., &; Elbert, T. (2000). EEG brain mapping of phonological and semantic tasks in Italian and German languages. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(4), 706-716.

Beaton, A. A. (1997). The relation of planum temporale asymmetry and morphology of the corpus callosum to handedness, gender, and dyslexia: a review of the evidence. Brain and Language, 60(2), 255-322.

Beery, K. E., (1997). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration with supplemental developmental tests of visual perception and motor coordination: administration, scoring, and teaching manual (4th ed.), Parsippany, NJ: Modern Curriculum Press.

Bender, S., Weisbrod, M., Bornfleth, H., Resch, F., &; Oelkers-Ax, R. (2005). How do children prepare to react? Imaging maturation of motor preparation and stimulus anticipation by late contingent negative variation. Neuroimage, 27(4), 737.

Bodiker, G. M. (2004). fMRI Analysis of Inverted and Non-inverted Left-handed Subjects During Language Tasks. Doctoral dissertation, University of Toledo Health Science Campus.

Brang, D., Kanai, S., Ramachandran, V. S., &; Coulson, S. (2011). Contextual priming in grapheme–color synesthetes and yoked controls: 400 msec in the life of a synesthete. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1681-1696.

Butler, S. R., Glass, A., &; Heffner, R. (1981). Asymmetries of the contingent negative variation (CNV) and its after positive wave (APW) related to differential hemispheric involvement in verbal and non-verbal tasks. Biological Psychology, 13, 157-171.

Buxbaum, L. J., Coslett, H. B., Schall, R. R., McNally, B., &; Goldberg, G. (1993). Hemispatial factors in mirror writing. Neuropsychologia, 31(12), 1417-1421.

Cao, X. H., &; Zhang, H. T. (2011). Change in subtle N170 specialization in response to Chinese characters and pseudocharacters. Perceptual and motor skills, 113(2), 365-376.

Cao, X., Li, S., Zhao, J., Lin, S., &; Weng, X. (2011). Left-lateralized early neurophysiological response for Chinese characters in young primary school children. Neuroscience Letters, 492(3), 165-169.

Chan, J. L., &; Ross, E. D. (1988). Left-handed mirror writing following right anterior cerebral artery infarction: Evidence for nonmirror transformation of motor programs by right supplementary motor area. Neurology, 38(1), 59-63.

Coch, D., Grossi, G., Skendzel, W., &; Neville, H. (2005). ERP nonword rhyming effects in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 168-182.

Coch, D., Hart, T., &; Mitra, P. (2008). Three kinds of rhymes: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 104(3), 230-243.

Cornell J. (1985). Spontaneous mirror-writing in children. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 174-179.

Cubelli, R., &; Della-Sala, S. (2009). Mirror writing in pre-school children: A pilot study. Cognitive Process, 10(2), 101-104.

Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. Penguin Books.

Dehaene, S., Nakamura, K., Jobert, A., Kuroki, C., Ogawa, S., &; Cohen, L. (2010). Why do children make mirror errors in reading? Neural correlates of mirror invariance in the visual word form area. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1837-1848.

Demiral, S. B., Malcolm, G. L., &; Henderson, J. M. (2012). ERP correlates of spatially incongruent object identification during scene viewing: contextual expectancy versus simultaneous processing. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1271-1285.

Donchin, E., Ritter, W., &; McCallum, W.C. (1978). Cognitive psychophysiology: The endogenous components of the ERP. In E. Callaway, P. Tueting, &; S. Koslow (Eds.), Event-related brain potentials in man (pp. 349-358). New York: Academic Press.

Duncan, C. C., Barry, R. J., Connolly, J. F., Fischer, C., Michie, P. T., Naatanen, R., . . .&; Van Petten, C. (2009). Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, and quantifying mismatch negativity, P300, and N400. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(11), 1883-1908.

Fischer, J. P. &; Tazouti, Y. (2012). Unraveling the mystery of mirror writing in typically developing children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 193-205.

Giedd, J. N., Rumsey, J. M., Castellanos, F. X., Rajapakse, J. C., Kaysen, D., Catherine Vaituzis, A., ... &; Rapoport, J. L. (1996). A quantitative MRI study of the corpus callosum in children and adolescents. Developmental Brain Research, 91(2), 274-280.

Gordon, H. (1921). Left-handedness and mirror writing, especially among defective children. Brain, 43(4), 313-368.

Gottfried, J. A., Sancar, F., &; Chatterjee, A. (2003). Acquired mirror writing and reading: Evidence for reflected graphemic representations. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), 96-107.

Hamm, J. P., Johnson, B. W., &; Corballis, M. C. (2004). One good turn deserves another: An event-related brain potential study of rotated mirror–normal letter discriminations. Neuropsychologia, 42(6), 810-820.

Heilman, K. M., Howell, G., Valenstein, E., &; Rothi, L. (1980). Mirror-reading and writing in association with right-left spatial disorientation. Journal Neurology Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 43(9), 774-780.

Henderson, L. M., Baseler, H. A., Clarke, P. J., Watson, S., &; Snowling, M. J. (2011). The N400 effect in children: Relationships with comprehension, vocabulary and decoding. Brain and language, 117(2), 88-99.

Jucla, M., Nenert, R., Chaix, Y., &; Demonet, J. F. (2010). Remediation effects on N170 and P300 in children with developmental dyslexia. Behavioural Neurology, 22(3-4), 121-129.

Katz, N., Golstand, S., Bar-Ilan, R., &; Parush, S. (2007). The Dynamic Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment for Children (DOTCA–Ch): A New Instrument for Assessing Learning Potential. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61(1), 41.

Kloppel, S., Mangin, J. F., Vongerichten, A., Frackowiak, R. S., &; Siebner, H. R. (2010). Nurture versus nature: Long-term impact of forced right-handedness on structure of pericentral cortex and basal ganglia. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(9), 3271-3275.

Kloppel, S., Vongerichten, A., van Eimeren, T., Frackowiak, R. S., &; Siebner, H. R. (2007). Can left-handedness be switched? Insights from an early switch of handwriting. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(29), 7847-7853.

Knecht, S., Dräger, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Flöel, A., ... &; Henningsen, H. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123(12),
2512-2518.

Kushnir, T., Arzouan, Y., Karni, A., &; Manor, D. (2013). Brain activation associated with practiced left hand mirror writing. Brain and Language, 125(1), 38-46.

Law, S.-P., &; Or, B. (2001). A Case Study of Acquired Dyslexia and Dysgraphia in Cantonese: Evidence for Nonsemantic Pathways for Reading and Writing Chinese. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(8), 729-748.

Lin, S. E., Chen, H. C., Zhao, J., Li, S., He, S., &; Weng, X. C. (2011). Left-lateralized N170 response to unpronounceable pseudo but not false Chinese characters—the key role of orthography. Neuroscience, 190, 200-206.

Lux, S., Keller, S., Mackay, C., Ebers, G., Marshall, J. C., Cherkas, L., ... &; Gurd, J. M. (2008). Crossed cerebral lateralization for verbal and visuo‐spatial function in a pair of handedness discordant monozygotic twins: MRI and fMRI brain imaging. Journal of anatomy, 212(3), 235-248.

Ma, H. F., Wang, P. P., &; Xu, G. F. (2011). The mirror error in reading and writing and its underlying mechanisms. Journal of Psychological Science, 34(3), 552-557.

Margolin, D. I. (1984). The neuropsychology of writing and spelling: Semantic, phonological, motor, and perceptual processes. The Quarterfy Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 459-489.

Marzi, C. A., Grabowska, A., Tressoldi, P., &; Bisiacchi, P. S. (1988). Left hemisphere superiority for visuospatial functions in left-handers. Behavioural brain research, 30(2), 183-192.

Matsuo, K., Kato, C., Ozawa, F., Takehara, Y., Isoda, H., Isogai, S., Moriya, T, Sakahara, H., Okada, T., &; Nakai, T. (2002). Manipulo-spatial processing of ideographic characters in left-handers: observation in fMRI. Magnetic Resonance Medical Sciences, 1(1), 21-26.

Maurer, U., Blau, V. C., Yoncheva, Y. N., &; McCandliss, B. D. (2010). Development of visual expertise for reading: rapid emergence of visual familiarity for an artificial script. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(4), 404-422.

Maurer, U., Brem, S., Bucher, K., &; Brandeis, D. (2005). Emerging neurophysiological specialization for letter strings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1532-1552.

Maurer, U., Brem, S., Kranz, F., Bucher, K., Benz, R., Halder, P., ... &; Brandeis, D. (2006). Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read. Neuroimage, 33(2), 749.

Mclntosh, R. D., &; Della Sala, S. (2012). Mirror-Writing. Psychologist, 25(10), 742-746.

Meng, L. F. (2007). The rate of handedness conversion and related factor in left-handed children. Laterality, 12(2), 131-138.

Meng, L. F., Lu, C. P., &; Chan, H. L. (2007). Time-varying brain potentials and interhemispheric coherences of anterior and posterior regions during repetitive unimanual finger movements. Sensors, 7, 960-978.

Meyers, J. E., &; Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Neuroscan. (2003). Stim2: A comprehensive library of sensory, cognitive and neuropsychological tasks. Texas: Author.

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113.

Orton, S. T. (1928). Specific reading disability- strephosymbolia. Journal of the American Medical Association, 90(14), 1095–1099.

Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing and speech problems in children. New York: Norton.

Pegado, F., Nakamura, K., Cohen, L., &; Dehaene, S. (2011). Breaking the symmetry: mirror discrimination for single letters but not for pictures in the Visual Word Form Area.
Neuroimage, 55(2), 742-749.

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology: official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128.

Porac, C., Coren, S., &; Searleman, A. (1986). Environmental factors in hand preference formation: evidence from attempts to switch the preferred hand. Behavior Genetics, 16(2), 251-261.

Portney, L. G., &; Watkins, M. P. (2009). Foundations of clinical research: Applications to practice (3rd ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall Healthy.

Proverbio, A. M., Wiedemann, F., Adorni, R., Rossi, V., Del Zotto, M., &; Zani, A. (2007). Dissociating object familiarity from linguistic properties in mirror word reading. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(43), 1-22.

Pujol, J., Deus, J., Losilla, J. M., &; Capdevila, A. (1999). Cerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology,52(5), 1038-1038.

Raney, G. E. (1993). Monitoring changes in cognitive load during reading: an event-related brain potential and reaction time analysis. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 19(1), 51-69.

Rapcsak, S. Z. &; Beeson, P. M. (2000). Agraphia. In Aphasia &; Language: Theory to Practice. In S. E. Nadeau, L. J. G. Rothi, &; B. Crosson (Eds.), Aphasia and Language (pp. 184-220). USY, NY: Guilford Press.

Roeltgen, D. P. (2003). Agraphia. In Heilman, K. M., &; Valenstein, E. (Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology, Fourth ed. (pp.127-141). NY: Oxford University Press.

Schott, G. D. (2007). Mirror writing: neurological reflections on an unusual phenomenon. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 78(1), 5-13.
Siebner, H. R., Limmer, C., Peinemann, A., Drzezga, A., Bloem, B. R., Schwaiger, M., &;

Conrad, B. (2002). Long-term consequences of switching handedness: a positron emission tomography study on handwriting in “converted”left-handers. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(7), 2816–2825.

Sitnikova, T., Holcomb, P. J., Kiyonaga, K. A., &; Kuperberg, G. R. (2008). Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(11), 2037-2057.

Sommer, I. E. C., Ramsey, N. F., Mandl, R. C. W., &; Kahn, R. S. (2002). Language lateralization in monozygotic twin pairs concordant and discordant for handedness. Brain, 125(12), 2710-2718.

Spironelli, C., &; Angrilli, A. (2006). Language lateralization in phonological, semantic and orthographic tasks: a slow evoked potential study. Behavioural Brain Research, 175(2), 296-304.

Strotzer, M. (2009). One Century of Brain Mapping Using Brodmann Areas. Clinical Neuroradiology, 19(3), 179-186.

Svojanovsky, A. (2002). Brain amp- MR: Operating instructions. Munich: Brain Products GmbH.

Szeligo, F., Brazier, B., &; Houston, J. (2003). Adaptations of writing posture in
response to task demands for left- and right-handers. Laterality, 8(3), 261-276.

Tan, L. H., Liu, H. L., Perfetti, C. A., Spinks, J. A., Fox, P. T., &; Gao, J. H. (2001). The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. Neuroimage, 13(5), 836-846.

Tankle, R. S., &; Heilman, K. M. (1983). Mirror writing in right-handers and in left-handers. Brain and Language, 19(1), 115-123.

Tecce, J.J., &; Cattanach, L. (1993). Contingent negative variation (CNV). In E. Neidermeyer &; Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, clinical applications, and related fields (pp. 887-910). Baltimore: Williams &; Wilkins.

Tucha, O., Aschenbrenner, S., &; Lange, K. W. (2000). Mirror writing and handedness. Brain and Language, 73(3), 432-441.

Tuncer, M. C., Hatipoglu, E. S., &; Ozates, M. (2005). Sexsual dimorphism and handedness
in the human corpus callosum based on magnetic resonance imageing. Radiologic Anatomy, 27, 254-259.

Urgen, B. A., Plank, M., Ishiguro, H., Poizner, H., &; Saygin, A. P. (2012). Temporal Dynamics of Action Perception: The Role of Biological Appearance and Motion Kinematics. 34th Annual Conference of Cognitive Science Society, Sapporo, Japan.

Vaid, J., &; Stiles-Davis, J. (1989). Mirror writing: an advantage for the left-handed? Brain and Language, 37(4), 616-627.

Walter, W. G. (1964). Slow potential waves in the human brain associated with expectancy, attention and decision. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 206(3), 309-322.

Weekes, B. &; Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 277-315.

Weintraub, N., &; Graham, S. (1998). Writing legibly and quickly: A study of children’s ability to adjust their handwriting to meet common classroom demands. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 13, 146–152.

Witelson, S. F. (1985). The brain connection: the corpus callosum is larger in left-handers. Science, 229(4714), 665-668.

Wong, A. C., Gauthier, I., Woroch, B., Debuse, C., &; Curran, T. (2005). An early electrophysiological response associated with expertise in letter perception. Cognitive, Affective, &; Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(3), 306-318.

Yang, M. J. (1997). Mirror writing in right-handers and in left-handers: a study using Chinese characters. Neuropsychologia, 35(11), 1491-1498.

Zarei, M., Johansen‐Berg, H., Smith, S., Ciccarelli, O., Thompson, A. J., &; Matthews, P. M. (2006). Functional anatomy of interhemispheric cortical connections in the human brain. Journal of Anatomy, 209(3), 311-320.

Zhang, M., Jiang, T., Mei, L., Yang, H., Chen, C., Xue, G., &; Dong, Q. (2011). It's a word: Early electrophysiological response to the character likeness of pictographs. Psychophysiology, 48(7), 950-959.

Zhang, Y., Yuan, J., Bao, B., &; Zhang, Q. (2008). The recognition potential and rotated Chinese characters. Brain research, 1233, 98-105.

連結至畢業學校之論文網頁點我開啟連結
註: 此連結為研究生畢業學校所提供,不一定有電子全文可供下載,若連結有誤,請點選上方之〝勘誤回報〞功能,我們會盡快修正,謝謝!
QRCODE
 
 
 
 
 
                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                               
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top
無相關期刊