中文部分
吳穎沺. (民92). 建構主義式的科學學習活動對國小高年級學生認知結構之影響. 碩士, 國立交通大學.吳穎沺. (民95). 高中生對於核能發電爭議之非制式推理思考-兼探網路探究活動之影響. 博士, 國立臺灣師範大學.李思屏、林晏州. (2001). 遊客對生態旅遊之環境態度與行爲關係之研究-以關渡自然公園爲例. 戶外遊憩研究, 14(3), 15-36.李權晃. (2007). 疾病內隱接受度、藥物內隱態度與服藥順從性之關係-以精神分裂症患者為例. 碩士, 天主教輔仁大學.汪炳宏. (民92). 國軍自我傷害內隱及外顯評量工具之發展與評析. 碩士 政治作戰學校.周子宇. (2009). 初探內隱聯結測驗在測量環境態度之應用. 碩士, 國立台灣師範大學.周泰安、邱發忠、陳學志、徐芝君及卓淑玲. (2009). 害羞的內隱與外顯測量. 教育心理學報, 41(2), 22.林村助. (民90). 我國高級職業學校機械群學生環保態度與認知之研究. 國立彰化師範大學.
邱發忠, 陳學志, 徐芝君, 吳相儀, & 卓淑玲. (2008). 內隱與外顯因素對創造作業表現的影響. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 50(2), 125-145.
邱發忠、陳學志、徐芝君、吳相儀&卓淑玲. (民97). 內隱與外顯因素對創造作業表現的影響. 中華心理學刊, 50(2), 21.柯俊欽. (民92). 南投縣國小學童環境知識、環境態度與環境行為意向之調查研究. 碩士, 國立台南大學.涂沅澂. (民91). 個體對同性戀所持態度之外顯測量與內隱測量比較. 碩士, 天主教輔仁大學.
張文禎. (2010). 國小學生休閒態度與休閒參與之研究--以屏東縣為例. 碩士, 國立屏東師範學院.張春興. (1991). 現代心理學. 台北市: 台灣東華.
張春興. (1994). 教育心理學: 三化取向的理論與實踐: 臺灣東華.
張春興. (2004). 教育心理學-三化取向的理論與實務. 台北: 台灣東華.
張智遠. (民90). 國小五年級學童鄉土知識與態度之研究---以高雄市前鎮區為例. 碩士, 國立屏東教育大學.陳學志、李威震、周泰安和卓淑玲. (2002). 以內隱連結測驗(IAT)測量國人自尊的可行性研究. 中國測驗學會測驗年刊, 49(2), 18.傅安弘、簡嘉靜. (98). 台灣地區大學生屬性對營養知識、飲食態度與飲食行為之影響. 台灣營養學會雜誌, 34(4), 142-154.
黃旭鈞、吳清山. (2000). 學校推動知識管理策略初探. 教育研究月刊, 77, 18-32.賴杏枝. (民101). 民眾對人工溼地態度及維護管理之研究-以彰化縣鹿港鎮舊鹿港溪人工溼地為例. 碩士, 逢甲大學.英文部分
Ajzen, I. (1989). Attitude structure and behavior. Attitude structure and function, 241-274.
Allen, R., & Reber, A. S. (1980). Very long term memory for tacit knowledge. Cognition, 8(2), 175-185.
Anderson, O. R., Randle, D., & Covotsos, T. (2001). The role of ideational networks in laboratory inquiry learning and knowledge of evolution among seventh grade students. Science Education, 85(4), 410-425.
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380-393.
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R.M. (1968). Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes. In K. W. S. Spence, & Janet T. (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89 - 195).
Banse, R. (1999). Automatic evaluation of self and significant others: Affective priming in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16(6), 803-821.
Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1988). Interactive tasks and the implicit‐explicit distinction. British journal of Psychology, 79(2), 251-272.
Blair , I. (2001). Implicit stereotypes and prejudice. Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition.
Bosson, J. K., Swann Jr, W. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 631.
Brauer, M., Wasel,W,& Niedenthal, P. (2000). Implicit and explicit components of prejudice. Rev. Gen. Psychol, 4, 79 -101.
Breckler, S. J. (1984). Empirical validation of affect, behavior, and cognition as distinct components of attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1191.
De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1998). An affective variant of the Simon paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 12(1), 45-62.
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami,K.,& Beach,K.R. (2001). Implicit and explicit attitudes: examination of the relationship between measures of intergroup bias. In R. Brown, & Gaertner, S.L (Ed.), Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes (pp. 175-197). Malden: Blackwell.
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 316-326.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review Psychology, 54, 297-327.
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual review of psychology, 54(1), 297-327.
Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes.
Gagne. (1985). Gifted and Talent : Reexamining a Reexamination of the Definitions. Gifted Child Quaterly, 29, 103-112.
Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 509.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological review, 102(1), 4.
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological review, 109(1), 3.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197.
Hays, R. D., Hayashi, T., & Stewart, A. L. (1989). A five-item measure of socially desirable response set. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 49(3), 629-636.
Hetts, J. J., Sakuma, M., & Pelham, B. W. (1999). Two roads to positive regard: Implicit and explicit self-evaluation and culture. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(6), 512-559.
Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Hetts, J. J. (2002). Name letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: Implicit egotism as self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(2), 170-177.
Koole, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(4), 669.
Liao, S. Y. (2012). The Application of Piaget and Bruner's Cognitive Developmental Theory in Children's Dance Teaching. The International Journal of Arts Education.
Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives of psychology.
McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Social Cognition, 20, 483-510.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological review, 84(3), 231.
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19(6), 625-666.
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math= male, me= female, therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44.
Novak, J. D., & Gowin, D. B. (1984). Learning how to learn: Cambridge University Press.
Osgood, C. E. (1957). The measurement of meaning (Vol. 47): University of Illinois Press.
Paulhus, D. L., & Reid, D. B. (1991). Enhancement and denial in socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 307.
Perugini, M., & Banse, R. (2007). Personality, Implicit Self-concept and Automaticity. European Journal of Personality, 21, 257-261. doi: 10.1002
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion Communication and Persuasion (pp. 1-24): Springer.
Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2002). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward high-fat foods in obesity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 3, 517-521.
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 743-762.
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee, D. E. (2001). Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup share desirable traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(9), 1164-1178.
Sadler, T. D. (2004). Informal reasoning regarding socioscientific issues: A critical review of research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(5), 513-516.
Sears, D. O., Peplau, L. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social psychology(7th ed). Englewood Cliffs: NJ: Prentice Hall.
Shavelson, R. J. (1974). Methods for examining representations of A subject-matter structure in a student's memory. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 11(3), 231-249.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological review, 84(2), 127.
Shuell, T. J. (1985). Knowledge representation, cognitive structure and school learning: a historical perspective. Cognitive structure and conceptual change, 117-132.
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and social psychology review, 4(2), 108-131.
Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. (1958). Attitudes established by classical conditioning. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57(1), 37.
Stacy, A. W., Leigh, B. C., & Weingardt, K. (1997). An individual-difference perspective applied to word association. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(3), 229-237.
Stowman, S. A., & Donohue, B. (2005). Assessing child neglect: A review of standardized measures. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10(4), 491-512.
Teachman, B. A., Gregg, A. P., & Woody, S. R. (2001). Implicit associations for fear-relevant stimuli among individual with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 226-235.
Thurstone, L. L. (1929). Theory of attitude measurement. Psychological review, 36(3), 222.
Tsai, C.-C., & Huang, C.-M. (2002). Exploring students' cognitive structures in learning science: a review of relevant methods. Journal of Biological Education, 36(4), 163-169.
Tsai, C. C. (1999). Content analysis of Taiwanese 14 year olds’ information processing operations shown in cognitive structures following physics instruction, with relations to science attainment and scientific epistemological beliefs. Research in science & technological education, 17(2), 125-138.
Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1968). A study of learning and retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 6(3), 283-291.
West, L. H. T., Fensham, P.J.,& Garrard, J.E. (1985). describing the cognitive structure of learners flowing instruction in chemistry. Cognitive structure and conceptual change, 29-48.
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41-78.
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological review, 107(1), 101.
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 262.
Wood, D., Bruner, J.S. ,&Ross, G. . (1976). THE ROLE OF TUTORING IN PROBLEM SOLVING. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(2), 89-100.
Woolfolk, A. (2004). Educational psychology(9th ed.). Boston,MA: Allyn & Bacon