跳到主要內容

臺灣博碩士論文加值系統

(18.97.9.172) 您好!臺灣時間:2024/12/03 07:05
字體大小: 字級放大   字級縮小   預設字形  
回查詢結果 :::

詳目顯示

我願授權國圖
: 
twitterline
研究生:曾香寧
研究生(外文):Shiang-Ning Tseng
論文名稱:完美,並不美:消費者類型對規範框架訊息在無為慣性效果上的影響
論文名稱(外文):Who will Skip this Deal: Maximizer or Satisfier?
指導教授:葉明義葉明義引用關係
指導教授(外文):Min-Yih Yeh
口試委員:吳宗祐謝亦泰
口試日期:2017-07-05
學位類別:碩士
校院名稱:國立臺灣科技大學
系所名稱:企業管理系
學門:商業及管理學門
學類:企業管理學類
論文種類:學術論文
論文出版年:2017
畢業學年度:105
語文別:中文
論文頁數:42
中文關鍵詞:無為慣性極大化者與滿足化者規範框架訊息適配預期後悔
外文關鍵詞:Inaction inertiaMaximizer/SatisficerRegulatory-Framed MessageFitAnticipated regret
相關次數:
  • 被引用被引用:0
  • 點閱點閱:281
  • 評分評分:
  • 下載下載:0
  • 收藏至我的研究室書目清單書目收藏:0
當今琳琅滿目的降價促銷,不是每個人都能把握住每次的優惠,錯過優惠對消費者而言是普遍的事。消費者錯過較佳優惠後,再面對次佳優惠時,採取行動的可能性會降低,被稱為無為慣性。本研究主旨在探討極大化與滿足化的這兩種類型的消費者錯過優惠後,採取次佳優惠的可能性。我們用四個實驗驗證以下關係:當錯過較佳優惠,面臨次佳優惠時,滿足化者比極大化者的購買意願為高。此種型態僅發生在兩次優惠相對吸引力差距大時。當產品訊息以規範焦點呈現時,在促進框架訊息下,滿足化者比極大化者表現出的無為慣性程度較小;在預防框架訊息下,兩者的無為慣性程度無顯著差異。此干擾效果的產生,源自於消費者與規範訊息適配所導致預期後悔的減輕,亦即,訊息以促進框架呈現時,與之適配的滿足化者所產生的預期後悔較非適配的極大化者為輕,因而前者無為慣性程度會低於後者;訊息以預防框架呈現時,與之適配的極大化者因適配降低所產生的預期後悔,使得無為慣性程度降至與滿足化者相似。
In spite of numerous price-discount promotions every day, not every consumer is able to seal the best deal. It is common for people to overlook promotion events in daily life. The phenomenon that one is not likely to act upon the less attractive opportunity after bypassing the more attractive opportunity is called inaction inertia. This research aims to investigate how likely maximizers and satisifisers will act on the inferior promotion after missing the superior promotion. We use four experiments to test following relationship: First, maximizers show higher inaction inertia than satisficers do. Second, this phenomenon only happens when the difference in attractiveness between the missed and the current opportunity is large. Besides, regulatory focus theory can be applied to message framing. Under promotion message framing, satisfiers show weaker inaction inertia than maximizers. On the other hand, no difference in inaction inertia exists between maximizers and satisficers under prevention message framing. Moreover, we look at the process underlying the difference in inaction inertia between consumer type and regulatory-framed message. When there is a fit between consumer type and regulatory-framed message, it will lead to lower regret.
目錄
摘要I
AbstractII
第一章 緒論1
第二章 文獻探討3
第一節 極大化與滿足化者3
第二節 無為慣性4
第三節 無為慣性與經驗後悔、預期行動後悔、預期不行動後悔4
第四節 極大化、滿足化者與無為慣性5
第三章 實驗一:極大化者及滿足化者的無為慣性程度8
第一節 方法8
第二節 結果8
第三節 結論9
第四章 實驗二:以相對吸引力差距小為邊界條件11
第一節 方法11
第二節 結果12
第三節 結論13
第五章 實驗三:規範框架訊息和消費者類型對行動可能性17
第一節 方法17
第二節 結果18
第三節 結論20
第六章 實驗四:預期後悔的中介效果21
第ㄧ節 前測21
第二節 方法21
第三節 結果22
第四節 結論26
第七章 結論與建議27
第一節 理論貢獻27
第二節 管理意涵28
第三節 研究限制與未來研究建議29
附錄30
實驗ㄧ:極大化量表(Maximization Scale)(Schwartz et al. 2002)30
實驗二:極大化傾向量表(Maximizing Tendency Scale)(Diab, Gillespie, and Highhouse 2008)30
參考文獻31


第一章 緒論
Aaker, Jennifer L. and Angela Y. Lee (2001), ““I” Seek Pleasures and “We” Avoid Pains: The Role of Self-Regulatory Goals in Information Processing and Persuasion,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (June), 33‒48.
Arkes, Hal R., Yi Han Kung, and Laura Hutzel (2002), “Regret, Valuation, and Inaction Inertia,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 87 (2), 371‒385.
Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator–Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.
Bulter, Adam and Scott Highhouse (2000), “Deciding to Sell: The Effect of Prior Inaction and Offer Source,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 21 (3), 223‒232.
Cheek, Nathan N. and Barry Schwartz (2015), “On the Meaning and Measurement of Maximization,” Judgment and Decision Making, 11 (2), 126‒146.
Chernev, Alexander (2004), “Goal Orientation and Consumer Preference for the Status Quo,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (3), 557‒565.
Chowdhury, Tilottama G., S. Ratneshwar, and Praggyan Mohanty (2009), “The Time-harried Shopper: Exploring the Differences between Maximizers and Satisficers,” Markering Letters, 20(2), 155‒167.
Dar-Nimrod, Ilan, Catherine D. Rawn, Darrin R. Lehman, and Barry Schwartz (2009), “The Maximization Paradox: The Costs of Seeking Alternatives,” Personality and Individual Differences, 46 (5), 631‒635.
Diab, Dalia L., Michael A. Gillespie, and Scott Highhouse (2008), “Are Maximizers Really Unhappy? The Measurement of Maximizing Tendency,” Judgement and Decision Making, 3 (5), 364‒370.
Dieffendorff, James M., Rosalie J. Hall, Robert G. Lord, and Mona L. Strean (2000), “Action-state Orientation: Construct Validity of a Revised Measure and Its Relationship to Work-related variables,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 85 (2), 250‒263.
Finkelstein, Stacey R. and Ayelet Fishbach (2012), “Tell Me What I Did Wrong: Experts Seek and Respond to Negative Feedback,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (1), 22‒38.
Friedman, Ronald S. and Jens Förster (2000), “The Effects of Approach and Avoidance Motor Actions on the Elements of Creative Insight,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (4), 477‒492.
Hayes, Andrew F. (2012), “PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling,” http:// www.afhayes.com/introduction-to-mediation-moderationand-conditional-process-analysis.html#process.
Higgins, E. Tory (1987), “Self-discrepancy: A Theory Relating Self and Affect,” Psychological Review, 94 (3), 319‒340.
Higgins, E. Tory (2000), “Making a Good Decision: Value from Fit,” American Psychologist, 55 (11), 1217‒1230.
Higgins, E. Tory, Lorraine Chen Idson, Antonio L. Freitas, Scott Spiegel, and Daniel C. Molden (2003), “Transfer of Value from Fit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (6),1140‒1153.
Inman, J. Jeffrey (2007), “Regret Regulation: Disentangling Self-Reproach from Learning,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17 (1), 19‒24
Itzkin, Adi, Dina Van Dijk, and Ofer H. Azar (2016), “At Least I Tried: The Relationship between Regulatory Focus and Regret Following Action vs. Inaction,” Frontiers in psychology, 7 (October).
Iyengar, Sheena S., Rachel E. Wells, and Barry Schwartz (2006), “Doing Better but Feeling Worse: Looking for the “Best” Job Undermines Satisfaction,” Psychological Science, 17 (2), 143‒150.
Kumar, Piyush (2004), “The Effects of Social Comparison on Inaction Inertia,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95(2), 175‒185.
Lai, Linda (2010), “Maximizing Without Difficulty: A Modified Maximizing Scale and its Correlates,” Judgment and Decision Making, 5 (3), 164‒175.
Misuraca, Raffaella, Palmira Faraci, Amelia Gangemi, Floriana A. Carmeci and Silvana Miceli (2015), “The Decision Making Tendency Inventory: A New Measure to Assess Maximizing, Satisficing, and Minimizing,” Personality and Individual Differences, 85 (October), 111‒116.
Misuraca, Raffaella, Ursina Teuscher, and Floriana Antonella Carmeci (2015), “Who are Maximizer Future Oriented and Highly Numerate individuals,” International Journal of Psychology, 51 (4), 307‒311.
Ma, Jingjing and Neal J. Roese (2014), “The Maximizing Mind-Set,” Journal of Consumer Research, 41 (1), 71‒92.
Mao, Wen (2016), “When One Desires Too Much of a Good Thing: The Compromise Effect under Maximizing Tendencies,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26 (1), 66‒80.
Micu, Camelia C. and Tilottama G. Chowdhury (2010), “The Effect of Message's Regulatory Focus and Product Type on Persuasion,” Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 18 (2), 181‒190.
Mogilner, Cassie, Jennifer L. Aaker, and Ginger L. Pennington (2008), “Time Will Tell: The Distant Appeal of Promotion and Imminent Appeal of Prevention,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (February), 670‒681.
Nenkov, Gergana Y., Maureen Morrin, Andrew Ward, Barry Schwartz and John Hulland (2008), “A Short Form of the Maximization Scale: Factor Structure, Reliability and Validity Studies,” Judgment and Decision Making, 3 (5), 371‒388.
Pham, Michel Tuan and Tamar Avnet (2004), “Ideals and Oughts and the Reliance on Affect versus Substance in Persuasion,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (4), 503‒518.
Polman, Evan (2012), “Self–other Decision Making and Loss Aversion,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119 (2), 141‒150.
Roets, Arne, Barry Schwartz and Yanjun Guan (2012), “The Tyranny of Choice: A Cross-cultural Investigation of Maximizing-satisficing Effects on Well-being,” Judgment and Decision Making, 7 (6), 689‒704.
Schwartz, Barry, Andrew Ward, John Monterosso, Sonja Lyubomirsky, Katherine White, and Darrin R. Lehman (2002), “Maximizing Versus Satisficing: Happiness Is a Matter of Choice,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (5), 1178‒1197.
Sevdails, Nick, Nigel Harvey, and Michelle Yip (2006), “Regret Triggers Inaction Inertia‒But Which Regret and How?” British Journal of Social Psychology, 45 (12), 839‒853.
Shiner, Rebecca L. (2015), “Maximizers, Satisficers, and Their Satisfaction With and Preferences for Reversible Versus Irreversible Decisions,” Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6 (8), 896‒903.
Sparks, Erin A., Joyce Ehrlinger, and Richard P. Beth (2012), “Failing to Commit: Maximizers Avoid Commitment in a Way That Contributes to Reduced Satisfaction,” Personality and Individual Difference, 52 (1), 72‒77.
Summers, Christopher A., Robert W. Smith, and Rebecca Walker Reczek (2016), “An Audience of One: Behaviorally Targeted Ads as Implied Social Labels,” Journal of Consumer Research, 43 (1), 156‒178.
Thaler, Richard (1985), “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,” Marketing Science, 4 (3), 199‒214.
Tsiros, Michael (2009), “Releasing the Regret Lock: Response to New Alternatives after a Sale,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (April), 1039‒1059.
Turner, Brandon M., Hye Bin Rim, Nancy E. Betz, and Thomas E. Nygren (2012), “The Maximization Inventory,” Judgment and Decision Making, 7 (1), 48‒60.
Tykocinski, Orit E. and Thane S. Pittman (1998), “The Consequences of Doing Nothing: Inaction Inertia as Avoidance of Anticipated Counterfactual Regret,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (3), 607‒616.
Tykocinski, Orit E., Thane S. Pittman, and Erin E. Tuttle (1995), “Inaction Inertia: Foregoing Future Benefits as a Result of an Initial Failure to Act,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 (5), 793‒803.
Tykocinski, Orit E. and Thane S. Pittman (2001) “Product Aversion Following a Missed Opportunity: Price Contrast or Avoidance of Anticipated Regret?” Basic and Applied Social Psychology 23 (3), 149‒156.
Van Putten, Marijke, Marcel Zeelenberg, and Eric Van Dijk (2007), “Decoupling the Past from the Present Attenuates Inaction Inertia,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20 (1), 65‒79.
Van Putten, Marijke, Marcel Zeelenberg, and Eric Van Dijk (2008), “Multiple Options in the Past and the Present: The Impact on Inaction Inertia,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21 (5), 519‒531.
Van Putten, Marijke, Marcel Zeelenberg, and Eric Van Dijk (2009), “Dealing with Missed Opportunities: Action vs. State Orientation Moderates Inaction Inertia,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45 (4), 808‒815.
Van Putten, Marijke, Marcel Zeelenberg, and Eric Van Dijk (2010), “Who Throws Good Money after Bad? Action vs. State Orientation Moderates the Sunk Cost Fallacy,” Judgment and Decision Making, 5 (1), 33‒36.
Van Putten, Marijke, Marcel Zeelenberg, and Eric Van Dijk (2013), “How Consumers Deal with Missed Discounts: Transaction Decoupling, Action Orientation and Inaction Inertia,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 38 (October), 104‒110.
Weaver, Kimberlee, Kim Daniloski, Norbert Schwartz and Keenan Cottone (2015), “The Role of Social Comparison for Maximizers and Satisficers: Wanting the Best or Wanting to be the Best,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25 (3), 372‒388.
Werth, Lioba and Jens Foerster (2007), “How Regulatory Focus Influences Consumer Behavior,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(1), 33-51.
Zeelenberg, Marcel and Marijke Van Putten (2005), “The Dark Side of Discounts: An Inaction Inertia Perspective on the Post-promotion Dip,” Psychology & Marketing, 22 (8), 611‒622.
Zeelenberg, Marcel, Bernard A. Nijstad, Marijke Van Putten, and Eric Van Dijk (2006), “Inaction Inertia, Regret, and Valuation: A Closer Look,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 101 (1), 89‒104.
Zhou, Rongrong and Michel Tuan Pham (2004), “Promotion and Prevention across Mental Accounts: When Financial Products Dictate Consumers’ Investment Goals,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (1), 125‒135.
Zhu, Rui and Joan Meyers-Levy (2007), “Exploring the Cognitive Mechanism that Underlies Regulatory Focus Effects,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 89‒96.
Zimbardo, Philip G. and John N. Boyd (1999), “Putting Time in Perspective: A valid, Reliable Individual-Differences Metric,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (6), 1271‒1288.
連結至畢業學校之論文網頁點我開啟連結
註: 此連結為研究生畢業學校所提供,不一定有電子全文可供下載,若連結有誤,請點選上方之〝勘誤回報〞功能,我們會盡快修正,謝謝!
QRCODE
 
 
 
 
 
                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                               
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top