|
[1] 陳學志, 李威震, 周泰安, & 卓淑玲. (2002). 以內隱聯結測驗< IAT> 測量國人自尊的可行性研究. 測驗年刊, 49(2), 217-234. [2] A. Bargh, J. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life (Vol. 10). [3] Akitsuki, Y., & Decety, J. (2009). Social context and perceived agency affects empathy for pain: An event-related fMRI investigation. Neuroimage, 47(2), 722-734. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.091 [4] Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Chiavarino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004). Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci, 16(10), 1773-1784. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947928 [5] Aquino, K., & Reed, A., 2nd. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. J Pers Soc Psychol, 83(6), 1423-1440. [6] Bachevalier, J., & Loveland, K. A. (2006). The orbitofrontal–amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social–emotional behavior in autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(1), 97-117. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.002 [7] Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., & Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(4), 303-312. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm029 [8] Baumert, A., Rothmund, T., Thomas, N., Gollwitzer, M., & Schmitt, M. (2013). Justice as a moral motive: Belief in a just world and justice sensitivity as potential indicator of the justice motive (pp. 159-180). [9] Baumgartner, T., Fischbacher, U., Feierabend, A., Lutz, K., & Fehr, E. (2009). The neural circuitry of a broken promise. Neuron, 64(5), 756-770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.017 [10] Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Ladavas, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci, 2(2), 84-92. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm001 [11] Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci, 16, 1348. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470 https://www.nature.com/articles/nn.3470#supplementary-information [12] Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353-380. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006 [13] Decety, J., & Michalska, K. J. (2010). Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. Dev Sci, 13(6), 886-899. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00940.x [14] Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., & Akitsuki, Y. (2008). Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children. Neuropsychologia, 46(11), 2607-2614. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.026 [15] Decety, J., Skelly, L. R., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(6), 638-645. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27 [16] Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nat Neurosci, 8(11), 1611-1618. doi: 10.1038/nn1575 [17] FeldmanHall, O., Mobbs, D., Evans, D., Hiscox, L., Navrady, L., & Dalgleish, T. (2012). What we say and what we do: the relationship between real and hypothetical moral choices. Cognition, 123(3), 434-441. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.001 [18] Fumagalli, M., & Priori, A. (2012). Functional and clinical neuroanatomy of morality. Brain, 135(Pt 7), 2006-2021. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr334 [19] Gaesser, B., Hirschfeld-Kroen, J., Wasserman, E. A., Horn, M., & Young, L. (2019). A role for the medial temporal lobe subsystem in guiding prosociality: the effect of episodic processes on willingness to help others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(4), 397-410. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz014 [20] Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105-2108. doi: 10.1126/science.1062872 [21] Greenwald, A., Nosek, B., & Banaji, M. (2003). Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: I. An Improved Scoring Algorithm (Vol. 85). [22] Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol, 74(6), 1464-1480. [23] J Van Bavel, J., FeldmanHall, O., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). The neuroscience of moral cognition: From dual processes to dynamic systems (Vol. 6). [24] Janoff-Bulman, R., Sheikh, S., & Hepp, S. (2009). Proscriptive versus prescriptive morality: two faces of moral regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol, 96(3), 521-537. doi: 10.1037/a0013779 [25] Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908-911. doi: 10.1038/nature05631 [26] Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492-527. [27] Luo, Q., Nakic, M., Wheatley, T., Richell, R., Martin, A., & Blair, R. J. (2006). The neural basis of implicit moral attitude--an IAT study using event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 30(4), 1449-1457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.005 [28] Mendez, M. F., Anderson, E., & Shapira, J. S. (2005). An investigation of moral judgement in frontotemporal dementia. Cogn Behav Neurol, 18(4), 193-197. [29] Narvaez, D., & Lapsley, D. K. (2009). Chapter 8 Moral Identity, Moral Functioning, and the Development of Moral Character. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 50, pp. 237-274): Academic Press. [30] Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol, 527 Pt 3, 633-639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x [31] Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology, 57(10), 1899-1901. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.10.1899 [32] Nosek, B. A., Bar-Anan, Y., Sriram, N., Axt, J., & Greenwald, A. G. (2014). Understanding and Using the Brief Implicit Association Test: Recommended Scoring Procedures. PLoS One, 9(12), e110938. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110938 [33] Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2012). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Curr Biol, 22(23), 2274-2277. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018 [34] Schmitt, M., Baumert, A., Gollwitzer, M., & Maes, J. (2010). The Justice Sensitivity Inventory: Factorial Validity, Location in the Personality Facet Space, Demographic Pattern, and Normative Data (Vol. 23). [35] Schmitt, M., Gollwitzer, M., Maes, J., & Arbach, D. (2005). Justice Sensitivity: Assessment and Location in the Personality Space (Vol. 21). [36] Schnabel, K., Banse, R., & Asendorpf, J. (2006). Employing automatic approach and avoidance tendencies for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept: The implicit association procedure (IAP). Exp Psychol, 53(1), 69-76. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.69 [37] Sellaro, R., Guroglu, B., Nitsche, M. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Massaro, V., Durieux, J., . . . Colzato, L. S. (2015). Increasing the role of belief information in moral judgments by stimulating the right temporoparietal junction. Neuropsychologia, 77, 400-408. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.09.016 [38] Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the implicit association test immune to faking? Exp Psychol, 51(3), 165-179. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.165 [39] Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2007). Moral hypocrisy: social groups and the flexibility of virtue. Psychol Sci, 18(8), 689-690. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01961.x [40] Yoder, K. J., & Decety, J. (2014). The Good, the bad, and the just: justice sensitivity predicts neural response during moral evaluation of actions performed by others. J Neurosci, 34(12), 4161-4166. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4648-13.2014 [41] Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 107(15), 6753-6758. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914826107 [42] Young, L., Cushman, F., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Hauser, M. (2006). Does Emotion Mediate the Relationship Between an Action's Moral Status and Its Intentional Status? Neuropsychological Evidence (Vol. 6). [43] Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(20), 8235-8240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701408104 [44] Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2009). Innocent intentions: a correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2065-2072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.020 [45] Zanto, T. P., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Fronto-parietal network: flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends in cognitive sciences, 17(12), 602-603. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.001
|