本土語言資源網:常見問答。取自:教育部本土語言資源網。https://mhi.moe.edu.tw/faqList.jsp
朱紹菱(2007)提升鄉土語言教學成效之行動研究〔未出版之碩士論文〕。國立東華大學教育研究所。何大安(2007)。語言活力通說。語言政策的多元文化思考,1(6)。
林生傳(1997)社會階層化與教育,教育社會學。
林信志、謝名娟、簡瑋成(2017)弱勢學生閩南語學習態度、口說能力與影響因素之研究,教育研究與發展期刊,13(1)。63–90。https://doi.org/10.3966/181665042017031301003
林緯倫、施依伶、張之倢(2020)雙語能力促進思考?執行功能扮演的可能角色,2020,第59屆年台灣心理學會。台灣,高雄。
高詩姮(2020)雙語平衡程度對執行功能及創造力的影響(未出版之碩士論文)。宜蘭:佛光大學心理系。陳榮華、陳心怡(2006)瑞文氏標準矩陣推理測驗提升本(SPM+)。中國行為科學社。
陳學志、陳彰儀、陳美芳、陳心怡、陳榮華(2011)華–葛氏批判思考量表精簡版(WGCTA)。中國行為科學社。
張光宇(2014)什麼是閩南語?台灣語文研究,9(1)。
張麗君(2008)國台雙語兒童其雙語能力與智力、創造力之相關探討。師大學報:教育類,53(1),79–106。
黃毅志(2003)「臺灣地區新職業聲望與社經地位量表」之建構與評估:社會科學與教育社會學研究本土化。教育研究集刊,49:4,1–31。
楊文山(2016)《105 年度全國客家人口暨語言基礎資料調查研究》。台北:行政會客家委員會。
楊素玲(2021)國小閩南語教學實施現狀與未來展望。臺灣教育評論月刊,10(10),147–151。
葉高華(2018)臺灣歷次語言普查回顧。臺灣語文研究,13(2),247–273。https://doi.org/10.6710/JTLL.201810_13(2).0004
劉珈妤(2016)透過執行功能訓練以提升思考力與創造力之探討(未出版之碩士論文)。宜蘭:佛光大學心理系。蕭素英(2012)閩南語「行、步、走」的語意地圖與其在華語區的傳播。臺灣語文研究,7(1),87–127。
蘇凰蘭(2019)在語言保存或流失的十字路口徘徊:台灣國中、小學生台語語言態度調查。臺灣語文研究,14(1),81–119。https://doi.org/10.6710/JTLL.201904_14(1).0003
Abutalebi, J., della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F., & Costa, A. (2012). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2076–2086. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr287
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta–analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. In Review of Educational Research (Vol. 80, Issue 2, pp. 207–245). https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310368803
Bagheri, F., & Ghanizadeh, A. (2016). Critical Thinking and Gender Differences in Academic Self–regulation in Higher Education. Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 3, 133–145.
Bailin, S., Case, R., Coombs, J. R., & Daniels, L. B. (1999). Conceptualizing critical thinking. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31(3). https://doi.org/10.1080/002202799183133
Balkan, L. (1970). Les effets du bilinguisme français–anglais sur les aptitudes intellectuelles. AImav.
Baron, J. (1985). Rationality and Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571275
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022–3514.51.6.1173
Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Dual–Process Theories of the Mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4). https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.130.4.553
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3). https://doi.org/10.1016/0010–0277(94)90018–3
Bialystok, E. (1987). Influences of bilingualism on metalinguistic development. Interlanguage Studies Bulletin (Utrecht), 3(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/026765838700300205
Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24(4). https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.24.4.560
Bialystok, E. (1997). Effects of bilingualism and biliteracy on children’s emerging concepts of print. Developmental Psychology, 33(3). https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.33.3.429
Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 60(1). https://doi.org/10.1037/cjep2006008
Bialystok, E. (2010). Global–Local and Trail–Making Tasks by Monolingual and Bilingual Children: Beyond Inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 93–105. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015466
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6). https://doi.org/10.1037/0278–7393.32.6.1341
Bialystok, E., Klein, R., Craik, F. I. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2004a). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882–7974.19.2.290
Bialystok, E., Klein, R., Craik, F. I. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2004b). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882–7974.19.2.290
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. In Developmental Science (Vol. 7).
Bialystok, Ellen. (2001). Bilingualism in development : language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007
Bloom, B. (1956). Taxonomy of Educational Objectives. Book I: Cognitive Domain.
Bochner, S. (1996). The learning strategies of bilingual versus monolingual students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(1), 83–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044–8279.1996.tb01178.x
Broca PP. (1861). Loss of speech, chronic softening and partial destruction of the anterior left lobe of the brain. . Bulletin de La Société Anthropologique., 2, 235–238.
Brookmeyer, R., Johnson, E., Ziegler–Graham, K., & Arrighi, H. M. (2007). Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 3(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2007.04.381
Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., de Lacy Costello, A., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38(6). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028–3932(99)00134–7
Burgess, P. W., & Wu, H.–C. (2013). Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Brodmann Area 10). In Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199837755.003.0037
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally Sensitive Measures of Executive Function in Preschool Children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2). https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3
Noam Chomsky. (1959). Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.
Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., Zald, D. H., & Olatunji, B. O. (2010). Emotion Modulation of Visual Attention: Categorical and Temporal Characteristics. PLoS ONE, 5(11). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013860
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Apesteguia, J., Heafner, J., & Keysar, B. (2014). Your Morals Depend on Language. PLoS ONE, 9(4). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094842
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa–Faidella, J., & Sebastián–Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The Five–Factor Model of Personality and Its Relevance to Personality Disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 6(4). https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1992.6.4.343
Cris Hamilton, A., & Martin, R. C. (2005). Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: A single–case study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.5.1.1
Cromdal, J. (1999). Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish–English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20(1). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716499001010
Cummins, J. (1976). The Influence of Bilingualism on Cognitive Growth: A Synthesis of Research Findings and Explanatory Hypotheses. . Working Papers on Bilingualism, 9, 1–43.
Cummins, James. (1978). Bilingualism and the Development of Metalinguistic Awareness. Journal of Cross–Cultural Psychology, 9(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/002202217892001
De Neys W. (2006). Dual processing in reasoning: two systems but one reasoner. Psychological science, 17(5), 428–433. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9280.2006.01723.x
Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Baddeley, A., Allamano, N., & Wilson, L. (1999). Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo–spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 37(10), 1189–1199. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028–3932(98)00159–6
Demie, F., & Strand, S. (2006). English language acquisition and educational attainment at the end of secondary school. Educational Studies, 32(2). https://doi.org/10.1080/03055690600631119
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1). https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev–psych–113011–143750
D’Souza, D., & D’Souza, H. (2016). Bilingual Language Control Mechanisms in Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Developmental Perspective. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(20). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0798–16.2016
Ennis, R. H., & Millman, J. (2005). Cornell Critical Thinking Test, Level X (5th ed.). The Critical Thinking Company.
Evans, J. ST. B. T. (1989). 4. Concepts and Inference. Mind & Language, 4(1–2). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468–0017.1989.tb00237.x
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual–Process Theories of Higher Cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3). https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
Facione, P. (1990). Critical Thinking: A Statement of Expert Consensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction (The Delphi Report). In Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC).
Facione, P. A. (2000). The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking: Its Character, Measurement, and Relationship to Critical Thinking Skill. Informal Logic, 20(1). https://doi.org/10.22329/il.v20i1.2254
Facione, P. A., & Facione, N. C. (1994). Critical thinking ability: A measurement tool. Assessment Update, 6(6). https://doi.org/10.1002/au.3650060611
Farhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V. (2010). Theory of Mind in Bilingual and Monolingual Preschool Children. Journal of Psychology, 1(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444
Fox, R., Corretjer, O., & Webb, K. (2019). Benefits of foreign language learning and bilingualism: An analysis of published empirical research 2012–2019. Foreign Language Annals, 52(4), 699–726. https://doi.org/10.1111/flan.12424
Fürst, G., & Grin, F. (2018). A comprehensive method for the measurement of everyday creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2018.03.007
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Racial differences in the intelligence of school children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9(5). https://doi.org/10.1037/h0073325
Greem, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico–semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000133
Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Perspective–taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive Development, 28(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.002
Gunning, C. S. (1981). Relationships among field inde– pendence, critical thinking ability, and clinical problem– solving ability of baccalaureate nursing students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42.
Halpern, D. (1999). Teaching for Critical Thinking: Helping College Students Develop the Skills and Dispositions of a Critical Thinker. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 1999, 69–74.
Halpern, D. F. (2010). Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment. . SCHUHFRIED.
Halpern, Diane F. (2014). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking, 5th ed. In Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking, 5th ed. Psychology Press.
Handley, S. J., Capon, A., Beveridge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. S. B. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 10(2). https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780442000051
Hartanto, A., Toh, W. X., & Yang, H. (2019). Bilingualism Narrows Socioeconomic Disparities in Executive Functions and Self–Regulatory Behaviors During Early Childhood: Evidence From the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Child Development, 90(4). https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13032
Hill, H. S. (1936). The Effect of Bilingualism on the Measured Intelligence of Elementary School Children of Italian Parentage. The Journal of Experimental Education, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1936.11010034
Janssen, E. M., Mainhard, T., Buisman, R. S. M., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Heijltjes, A. E. G., van Peppen, L. M., & van Gog, T. (2019). Training higher education teachers’ critical thinking and attitudes towards teaching it. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.03.007
Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 14, 89, 81.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive psychology, 21(1), 60–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010–0285(89)90003–0
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled–attention view of working–memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/0096–3445.130.2.169
Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011
Kenji Hakuta. (1986). Cognitive Development of Bilingual Children. Center for Language Educationand Research University of California.
Kevin N. Dunbar, & David Klahr. (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison, Eds.). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199734689.001.0001
Kharkhurin, A. V., & Wei, L. (2015). The role of code–switching in bilingual creativity. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 18(2), 153–169. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2014.884211
Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung–Beeman, M. (2006). The Prepared Mind. Psychological Science, 17(10). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9280.2006.01798.x
Ku, K. Y. L. (2009). Assessing students’ critical thinking performance: Urging for measurements using multi–response format. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2009.02.001
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta–analytic review. Psychological bulletin, 144(4), 394–425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142
Li, S., Ren, X., Schweizer, K., Brinthaupt, T. M., & Wang, T. (2021). Executive functions as predictors of critical thinking: Behavioral and neural evidence. Learning and Instruction, 71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2020.101376
Liedtke, W., & Nelson, L. D. (1968). Bilingualism and Conservation.
Lin, W.–L., Tsai, P.–H., Lin, H.–Y., & Chen, H.–C. (2014). How does emotion influence different creative performances? The mediating role of cognitive flexibility. Cognition and Emotion, 28(5). https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.854195
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5). https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574
Luk, G. C. (2008). The anatomy of the bilingual influence on cognition: Levels of functional use and proficiency of language. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 69(6–B), 3868.
Luo, J., Niki, K., & Phillips, S. (2004). The Function of the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) in the Insightful Solving of Puzzles: The ACC Is Activated Less When the Structure of the Puzzle Is Known. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 5(2), 195–213.
Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Rogers, R., Antoun, N., Aitken, M., & Robbins, T. (2002). Decision–making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain, 125(3). https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf049
Markovits, H., & Nantel, G. (1989). The belief–bias effect in the production and evaluation of logical conclusions. Memory & Cognition, 17(1). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199552
Meiran, N. (2010). Task Switching: Mechanisms Underlying Rigid vs. Flexible Self–Control. In Self Control in Society, Mind, and Brain. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195391381.003.0011
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Mohamed Zied, K., Phillipe, A., Karine, P., Valerie, H.–T., Ghislaine, A., Arnaud, R., & Gall Didier, L. (2004). Bilingualism and adult differences in inhibitory mechanisms: Evidence from a bilingual stroop task. Brain and Cognition, 54(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.036
Mohanty, A. K., & Babu, N. (1983). Bilingualism and Metalinguistic Ability among Kond Tribals in Orissa, India. The Journal of Social Psychology, 121(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1983.9924461
Mokhtari, K., & Reichard, C. (2004). Investigating the strategic reading processes of first and second language readers in two different cultural contexts. System, 32(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2004.04.005
Nicoladis, E., Hui, D., & Wiebe, S. A. (2018). Language Dominance and Cognitive Flexibility in French–English Bilingual Children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01697
Noone, C., Bunting, B., & Hogan, M. J. (2016). Does Mindfulness Enhance Critical Thinking? Evidence for the Mediating Effects of Executive Functioning in the Relationship between Mindfulness and Critical Thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02043
Noone, C., & Hogan, M. J. (2018). A randomised active–controlled trial to examine the effects of an online mindfulness intervention on executive control, critical thinking and key thinking dispositions in a university student sample. BMC Psychology, 6(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359–018–0226–3
Oakhill, J., Johnson–Laird, P. N., & Garnham, A. (1989). Believability and syllogistic reasoning. Cognition, 31(2). https://doi.org/10.1016/0010–0277(89)90020–6
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive psychology, 66(2), 232–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 69, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
Padilla, C., Mendez, M. F., Jimenez, E. E., & Teng, E. (2016). Bilingualism in older Mexican–American immigrants is associated with higher scores on cognitive screening. BMC Geriatrics, 16(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877–016–0368–1
Pang, M., Wong, K., Chan, K., Chan, M., Bai, H., Li, S., Li, S., Tsoi, Y., Wang, J., & Yin, L. (2004). Validity and reliability of the Chinese critical thinking disposition inventory. Chinese Journal of Nursing, 39(9), 644–647.
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligencxe. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(27). https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093840
Pintner, R., & Arsenian, S. (1937). The Relation of Bilingualism to Verbal Intelligence and School Adjustment. The Journal of Educational Research, 31(4). https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1937.10880748
Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253–262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990526
Quayle, J. D., & Ball, L. J. (2000). Working memory, metacognitive uncertainty, and belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 53(4). https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980050156362
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Raven Manual: Section 4, Advanced Progressive Matrices, 1998 Edition. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd.
Raven, P., P., F., Everard, M., H.T.H., H., & Dawson, F. (1997). River Habitat Survey: a new system to classify rivers according to their habitat quality. In Aquatic Conser6: Mar. Freshw. Ecosyst. (Vol. 8, pp. 215–234).
Rodriguez–Fornells, A., Rotte, M., Heinze, H.–J., Nösselt, T., & Münte, T. F. (2002). Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature, 415(6875). https://doi.org/10.1038/4151026a
Rubin, O., & Meiran, N. (2005). On the Origins of the Task Mixing Cost in the Cuing Task–Switching Paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6). https://doi.org/10.1037/0278–7393.31.6.1477
Sicam, F. P. M., & Lucas, R. I. G. (2016). Language attitudes of adolescent Filipino bilingual learners towards English and Filipino. Asian Englishes, 18(2). https://doi.org/10.1080/13488678.2016.1179474
Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. D. (1963). Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus–response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/00140136308930679
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1). https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.119.1.3
Spector, A., & Biederman, I. (1976). Mental Set and Mental Shift Revisited. The American Journal of Psychology, 89(4). https://doi.org/10.2307/1421465
Spoerl, D. T. (1944). The Academic and Verbal Adjustment of College Age Bilingual Students. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 64(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1944.10533261
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/0096–3445.127.2.161
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00003435
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6). https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651
Szczepanski, S. M., & Knight, R. T. (2014). Insights into Human Behavior from Lesions to the Prefrontal Cortex. Neuron, 83(5). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.011
Teng, E. L., Chui, H. C., Schneider, L. S., & Metzger, L. E. (1987). Alzheimer's dementia: Performance on the Mini–Mental State Examination. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 96–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022–006X.55.1.96
Teovanović, P. (2019). Dual processing in syllogistic reasoning: An individual differences perspective. Psihologijske Teme, 28(1), 125–145. https://doi.org/10.31820/pt.28.1.7
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The Trolley Problem. The Yale Law Journal, 94(6). https://doi.org/10.2307/796133
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2014). Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(2). https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.844729
Trilling, B., & Fadel, C. (2009). 21st century skills: Learning for life in our times. In 21st century skills: Learning for life in our times. Jossey–Bass/Wiley.
Tsujii, T., Sakatani, K., Masuda, S., Akiyama, T., & Watanabe, S. (2011). Evaluating the roles of the inferior frontal gyrus and superior parietal lobule in deductive reasoning: An rTMS study. NeuroImage, 58(2). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.076
Tsujii, T., & Watanabe, S. (2010). Neural correlates of belief–bias reasoning under time pressure: A near–infrared spectroscopy study. NeuroImage, 50(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.026
Tsujii, T., Yamamoto, E., Ohira, T., Takahashi, T., & Watanabe, S. (2010). Antihistamine effects on prefrontal cortex activity during working memory process in preschool children: A near–infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) study. Neuroscience Research, 67(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2010.01.010
Truscott, J., & Smith, M. S. (2019). The internal context of bilingual ㄍprocessing. John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/bpa.8
Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (1980). Watson–Glaser critical thinking appraisal. The Psychological Corporation.
Yntema, D. B. (1963). Keeping Track of Several Things at Once. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/001872086300500102
Yow, W. Q., & Li, X. (2015). Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00164
Zelazo, P. D., & Mller, U. (2002). Executive Function in Typical and Atypical Development. In Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Blackwell Publishers Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470996652.ch20