跳到主要內容

臺灣博碩士論文加值系統

(18.97.9.169) 您好!臺灣時間:2025/01/22 03:49
字體大小: 字級放大   字級縮小   預設字形  
回查詢結果 :::

詳目顯示

: 
twitterline
研究生:彭瑞鳳
研究生(外文):Jui-feng Peng
論文名稱:嗓音起始時間影響因素之探討-以國語和客語為研究對象
論文名稱(外文):Factors for Voice Onset Time: Stops in Mandarin and Hakka
指導教授:陳麗美陳麗美引用關係
指導教授(外文):Li-mei Chen
學位類別:碩士
校院名稱:國立成功大學
系所名稱:外國語文學系專班
學門:人文學門
學類:外國語文學類
論文種類:學術論文
論文出版年:2009
畢業學年度:97
語文別:英文
論文頁數:82
中文關鍵詞:聲調語言嗓音起始時間
外文關鍵詞:place of articulationlexical tonetone languagevoice onset timegendervowel context
相關次數:
  • 被引用被引用:1
  • 點閱點閱:454
  • 評分評分:
  • 下載下載:33
  • 收藏至我的研究室書目清單書目收藏:1
本研究主要為探討嗓音起始時間(voice onset time)之影響因素,藉由對國語和客語的六個塞爆音(stop) /p, t, k, p', t', k' /資料的收集,探討發音位置、相鄰母音、聲調和性別對嗓音起始時間的影響。

本研究的主要發現如下:
1. 發音位置、相鄰的母音、聲調和性別對嗓音起始時間均有顯著的影響。
2. 除了國語的送氣音,國語的不送氣音和客語均符合Cho and Ladefoged (1999) 所提出”發音位置越後面,嗓音起始時間愈長”的規則。本研究並進一步發現,後面的母音為後元音/u/時,國語和客語雙唇音之VOT值均長於齒齦音。此結果顯示,雖然塞爆音之VOT值會隨著發音部位愈後面而愈長,但該塞爆音和其相鄰母音相互作用的結果,仍會使其VOT值產生變異。
3. 當塞爆音後面跟隨著高元音/i, u/時,其嗓音起始時間將長於後面跟隨著低元音/a/的塞爆音。而當塞爆音後面跟隨著前元音/i/時,其嗓音起始時間將長於後面跟隨著後元音/u/的塞爆音,但國語不送氣音呈現相反的結果。
4. 聲調對塞爆音之VOT值有顯著的影響。客語的資料並顯示:當聲調為促音時,塞爆音之VOT值顯著的短於其他聲調。此外,分析國語的資料時,發現假字的存在使得VOT值明顯變長,也使得分析聲調影響的結果有所不同。所以,聲調對塞爆音之影響,仍有待進一步的研究探討。
5. 性別對塞爆音之嗓音起始時間有顯著的影響,男性念不送氣音之VOT值長於女性,而女性念送氣音之VOT值長於男性。此外,國語和客語的研究結果顯示:女性唸送氣音與不送氣音之差異均大於男性唸送氣音與不送氣音之差異。

由於本研究結果呈現發音位置、相鄰的母音、聲調和性別對嗓音起始時間都有顯著的影響,因此建議在未來的研究中應考量這些變項所可能造成的影響。尤其是研究聲調語言時,設計測試語料時應考量聲調所可能造成的影響。另外,不同性別的受試者念不送氣音和送氣音也有顯著的差異,因此建議將來的研究受試者男女的人數應相當,可避免其所可能造成的實驗偏誤。
This thesis examines the influence of four factors—place of articulation, vowel context, lexical tone, and gender—upon voice onset time (VOT) in Mandarin and Hakka. The examination of VOT values for Mandarin and Hakka word-initial stops /p, t, k, p', t', k'/ followed by three vowels /i, u, a/ in different lexical tones revealed that these factors have significant influences on the VOT values for stops. The results related to lexical tone are important as they suggest that future studies should take the influence of lexical tone into account when studying VOT values for stops in tonal languages. In addition, the results indicated that Hakka stops and Mandarin unaspirated stops conform to Cho and Ladefoged’s (1999) general agreement—namely, the further back the closure, the longer the VOT. However, Mandarin aspirated stops contradict this rule: the mean VOT for alveolar is shorter than that for bilabial. Furthermore, the phenomenon for Mandarin aspirated stops was caused by the interactions of the stop and its vowel context. That is, when followed by the vowel /u/, bilabial stops have longer mean VOTs than alveolar stops. As for the influence of vowel context, the results revealed that stops preceding high vowels have significantly longer mean VOT values than stops preceding low vowels, while stops followed by the front vowel have longer mean VOTs than stops followed by the back vowel, with the exception of Mandarin unaspirated stops. Finally, the results demonstrated that men produce longer mean VOTs for unaspirated stops than their female counterparts, but women produce longer mean VOTs for aspirated stops than male counterparts, suggesting that the number of male and female participants should be equal in future studies. The current study also determined that the distinctions of female speakers’ productions of unaspirated and aspirated stops are greater than the distinctions of male speakers’ productions in both languages.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Abstract (Chinese) i
Abstract (English) iii
Acknowledgements v
Table of Contents vi
List of Tables viii
List of Figures ix

CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION 1
1.1 Research Questions 3
1.2 Contribution of the Study 3

CHAPTER TWO LITERATURE REVIEW 5
2.1 Voice Onset Time (VOT) and Its Measurement 5
2.2 Factors Affecting Voice Onset Time 6
2.2.1 Place of Articulation 8
2.2.2 Vowel Context 14
2.2.3 Lexical Tone 17
2.2.4 Gender 20
2.3 The Features of Mandarin and Hakka 21

CHAPTER THREE METHODOLOGY 23
3.1 Participants 23
3.2 Corpus 24
3.3 Procedure 24
3.4 Data Measurement 25
3.5 Data Analysis 26

CHAPTER FOUR RESULTS 28
4.1 Mandarin Chinese 28
4.1.1 Place of Articulation and VOT 30
4.1.2 Vowel Context and VOT 33
4.1.3 Lexical Tone and VOT 38
4.1.4 Gender and VOT 42
4.2 Hakka 47
4.2.1 Place of Articulation and VOT 48
4.2.2 Vowel Context and VOT 52
4.2.3 Lexical Tone and VOT 57
4.2.4 Gender and VOT 60
4.3 Summary of the Results 64

CHAPTER FIVE DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION 68
Limitation of the Study and Suggestions for Future
Studies 75

REFERENCE 77
REFERENCES
Auzou, P., Ozsancak, C., Morris, R. J., Jan, M., Eustache, F. & Hannequin, D. (2000). Voice onset time in aphasia, apraxia of speech and dysarthria: a review. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 14(2), 131-150.
Byrd, D. (1992). Preliminary results on speaker-dependent variations in the TIMIT database. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92, 593-596.
Byrd, D. (1994). Relations of sex and dialect to reduction. Speech Communication, 15, 39-54.
Chao, K.-Y., Khattab, G. & Chen, L.- M. (2006). Comparison of VOT patterns in Mandarin Chinese and in English. Proceedings of the 4th Annual Hawaii International Conference on Arts and Humanities, 840–859.
Chao, Y. R. (1967). Mandarin Primer. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Chen, L.-M., Chao, K.-Y. & Peng, J.-F. (2007). VOT productions of word-initial stops in Mandarin and English: a cross-language study. Proceedings of the 19th Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing, 303-317.
Cheng, C.-C. (1973). A Synchronic Phonology of Mandarin Chinese. The Hague: Mouton.
Cho, T. & Keating, P. A. (2001). Articulatory and acoustic studies on domain-initial strengthening in Korean. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 155-190.
Cho, T. & Ladefoged, P. (1999). Variation and universals in VOT: evidence from 18 languages. Journal of Phonetics, 27, 207–29.
Chung, R.-F. (2004). An introduction to Taiwan Hakka phonology. Taipei: Wu-Nan Book Inc.
Fant, G. (1973). Speech sounds and features. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Francis, A. L., Ciocca, V. & Yu, J. M. C. (2003). Accuracy and variability of acoustic measures of voicing onset. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113 (2), 1025-1032.
Gosy, M. (2001). The VOT of the Hungarian voiceless plosives in words and in spontaneous Speech. International Journal of Speech Technology, 4, 75–85.
Gu, G.-S. (2005). Phonology of Taiwan Hakka. In Gu, G.-S. (Ed.), Introduction to Taiwan Hakka, 117-161. Taipei: Wu-Nan Book Inc.
Jancke, L. (1994). Variability and duration of voice onset time and phonation in stuttering and nonstuttering adults. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 19(1), 21-37.
Keating, P. A., Linker, W. & Huffman, M. (1983). Patterns in allophone distribution for voiced and voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 277–90.
Kehoe, M. M., Lleo, C., & Rakow, M. (2004). Voice onset time in bilingual German-Spanish children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(1), 71-88.
Kessinger, R. H. & Blumstein, S. E. (1997). Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time in Thai, French, and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 143 – 168.
Kessinger, R. H. & Blumstein, S. E. (1998). Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time and vowel production: some implications for perception studies. Journal of Phonetics, 26, 117-128.
Liao, S. J. (2005). Interlanguage production of English stop consonants: a VOT analysis. Unpublished master’s thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384–422.
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1967). Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language Speech, 10, 1-28.
Liu, H., Ng, M. L., Wan, M., Wang, S. & Zhang,Y. (2008). The effect of tonal changes on voice onset time in Mandarin esophageal speech. Journal of Voice, 22(2), 210-218.
Macleod, A. A. N. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2005). Are bilinguals different? What VOT tells us about simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3(2), 118-127.
Magloire, J. & Green, K. P. (1999). A cross-language comparison of speaking rate effects on the production of voice onset time in English and Spanish. Phonetica, 56, 158-185.
Morris, R. J., McCrea, C. R. & Herring, K. D. (2008). Voice onset time differences between adult males and females: isolated syllables. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 308–317.
Oh, E. (2009). Effects of speaking gender on voice onset time in Korean stops. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(4), 2574.
Port, R. F. (1979). Relation between voice-onset time and vowel duration. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66(3), 654-662.
Riney, T. J. & Takagi, N. (1999). Global foreign accent and voice onset time among Japanese EFL speakers. Language Learning, 49(2), 275–302.
Riney, T. J., Takagi, N., Ota, K. & Uchida, Y. (2007). The intermediate degree of VOT in Japanese initial voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 439-443.
Rochet, B. L. & Fei, Y. (1991). Effect of consonant and vowel context on Mandarin Chinese VOT: production and perception. Canadian Acoustics, 19(4), 105-106.
Rosner, B. S., López- Bascuas, L. E., García-Albea, J. E., & Fahey, R. P. (2000). Voice-onset times for Castilian Spanish initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 28, 217-224.
Ryalls, J., Zipprer, A. & Baldauff, P. (1997). Preliminary investigation of the effects of gender and race on voice onset time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 642-645.
Ryalls, J., Simon, M. & Thomason, J. (2004). Voice onset time production in older Caucasian- and African-Americans. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 2(1), 61–67.
Smith, B.L. (1978a). Temporal aspects of English speech production, a developmental perspective. Journal of Phonetics, 6, 37-67.
Smith, B.L. (1978b). Effects of place of articulation and vowel environment on voiced stop consonant production. Glossa, 12, 163-175.
Swartz, B.L. (1992). Gender differences in voice onset time. Percept. Mot. Skills, 75, 983-992.
Tsen, C.Y. (1994). An investigation on voice onset time and tone production of Chinese aphasia. Bulletin of the Institute of History and Philology Academia Sinica, 65(1), 37-79.
Whalen, D.H., Levitt, A. G. & Goldstein, L. M. (2007). VOT in the babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 341–352.
Whiteside, S. P. (1995). Temporal-based speaker sex differences in read speech: a sociophonetic approach. Proceedings of the 8th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 3, 516-519. Stockholm: KTH and Stockholm University.
Whiteside, S. P. (1996). Temporal-based acoustic phonetic patterns in read speech: some evidence for speaker sex differences. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 26, 23-40.
Whiteside, S. P. & Irving, C. J. (1997). Speakers’ sex differences in voice onset time: some preliminary findings. Percept Mot Skills, 85(2), 459-63.
Whiteside, S. P. & Irving, C. J. (1998). Speakers' sex differences in voice onset time: a study of isolated word production. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86(2), 651-4.
Whiteside, S.P. & Marshall, J. (2001). Developmental trends in voice onset time: some evidence for sex differences. Phonetica, 58, 196-210.
Whiteside, S. P., Henry, L. & Dobbin, R. (2004). Sex differences in voice onset time: a developmental study of phonetic context effects in British English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(2), 1179-1183.
Zheng, X.-R. & Li, Y.-H. (2005). A contrastive study of VOT of English and Korean stops. Journal of Yanbian University, 38(4), 99-102.
連結至畢業學校之論文網頁點我開啟連結
註: 此連結為研究生畢業學校所提供,不一定有電子全文可供下載,若連結有誤,請點選上方之〝勘誤回報〞功能,我們會盡快修正,謝謝!
QRCODE
 
 
 
 
 
                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                               
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top