|
Reference Academia Sinica. (1993). Word list with accumulated word frequency in news corpus [in Chinese]. Taipei, Taiwan: Association of Computational Linguistics and Chinese Language Processing. Anastasi, A., &; Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ, US: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Attix, D. K., Story, T. J., Chelune, G. J., Ball, J. D., Stutts, M. L., &; Hart, R. P. (2009). The prediction of change: Normative neuropsychological trajectories. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 21-38. Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Brinley, J. F. (1965). Cognitive sets, speed and accuracy of performance in the elderly. In A. T. Welford &; J. E. Birren (Eds.), Behavior, aging and the nervous system (pp. 114–149): Springfield, IL: Thomas. Bryan, J., &; Luszcz, M. A. (2000). Measures of fluency as predictors of incidental memory among older adults. Psychology and Aging, 15, 483-489. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.483 Bugg, J. M., Delosh, E. L., Davalos, D. B., &; Davis, H. P. (2007). Age differences in Stroop interference: Contributions of general slowing and task-specific deficits. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 155-167. Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67-83. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.1.67 Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information-processing rate. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press. Cerella, J. (1991). Age effects may be global, not local: Comment on Fisk and Rogers (1991). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 215-223. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.2.215 Chen, E. Y. H., Wong, A. W. S., Chen, R. Y. L., &; Au, J. W. Y. (2001). Stroop interference and facilitation effects in first-episode schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 29-44. Chen, J., Hale, S., &; Myerson, J. (2007). Predicting the size of individual and group differences on speeded cognitive tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin &; Review, 14, 534-541. doi: 10.3758/BF03194103 Chen, Y.-J., Ho, M.-Y., Chen, K.-J., Hsu, C.-F., &; Ryu, S.-J. (2009). Estimation of premorbid general fluid intelligence using traditional Chinese reading performance. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 63, 500-507. Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., &; Mcclelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332-361. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332 Daigneault, S., Braun, C. M., &; Whitaker, H. A. (1992). Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non-perseverative prefrontal measures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 99-114. Dash, J., &; Dash, A. S. (1982). Cognitive developmental studies of the Stroop phenomena: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Indian Psychologist, 1, 24-33. Deary, I. J., Der, G., &; Ford, G. (2001). Reaction times and intelligence differences: A population-based cohort study. Intelligence, 29, 389-399. doi: 10.1016/s0160-2896(01)00062-9 Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., &; Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., &; Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Demakis, G. J. (2004). Frontal lobe damage and tests of executive processing: A meta-analysis of the Category Test, Stroop test, and Trail-Making Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 441-450. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510149 Denney, D. R., &; Lynch, S. G. (2009). The impact of multiple sclerosis on patients' performance on the Stroop test: Processing speed versus interference. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 451-458. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090730 Denney, D. R., Lynch, S. G., Parmenter, B. A., &; Horne, N. (2004). Cognitive impairment in relapsing and primary progressive multiple sclerosis: Mostly a matter of speed. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 948-956. Denney, D. R., Sworowski, L. A., &; Lynch, S. G. (2005). Cognitive impairment in three subtypes of multiple sclerosis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 967-981. Dyer, F. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes. Memory &; Cognition, 1, 106-120. doi: 10.3758/BF03198078 Flanagan, D. P., &; Harrison, P. L. (2005). Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press. Goethals, I., Audenaert, K., Jacobs, F., Lannoo, E., Van De Wiele, C., Ham, H., . . . Dierckx, R. (2004). Cognitive neuroactivation using SPECT and the Stroop colored word test in patients with diffuse brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 21, 1059-1069. Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop color and word test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoeling. Graf, P., Uttl, B., &; Tuokko, H. (1995). Color- and picture-word Stroop tests: Performance changes in old age. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 390-415. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405132 Hale, S., &; Jansen, J. (1994). Global processing-time coefficients characterize individual and group differences in cognitive speed. Psychological Science, 5, 384-389. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00290.x Hale, S., Myerson, J., &; Wagstaff, D. (1987). General slowing of nonverbal information processing: Evidence for a power law. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 131-136. doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.2.131 Hartley, J. T. (1993). Aging and prose memory: Tests of the resource-deficit hypothesis. Psychology and Aging, 8, 538-551. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.4.538 Hasher, L., Lustig, C., &; Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake &; J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227-249). New York: Oxford University Press. Heaton, R. K., Ryan, L., &; Grant, I. (2009). Demographic influences and use of demographically corrected norms in neuropsychological assessment. In I. Grant &; K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric and neuromedical disorders (3rd ed., pp. 127-155). New York: Oxford University Press. Houx, P. J., Jolles, J., &; Vreeling, F. W. (1993). Stroop interference: Aging effects assessed with the Stroop color-word test. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 209-224. doi: 10.1080/03610739308253934 Huang, H. S. (2001). Chinese Graded Word Reading Test [in Chinese]. Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological Publishing. Hunt, E., &; Lansman, M. (1986). Unified model of attention and problem solving. Psychological Review, 93, 446-461. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.446 Kindt, M., Bierman, D., &; Brosschot, J. F. (1996). Stroop versus Stroop: Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard color-word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 653-661. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00133-X Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 576-588. Klein, M., Ponds, R. W., Houx, P. J., &; Jolles, J. (1997). Effect of test duration on age-related differences in Stroop interference. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 77-82. Lansbergen, M. M., Kenemans, J. L., &; Van Engeland, H. (2007). Stroop interference and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 21, 251-262. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., &; Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Lin, H. Y. (2013). An investigation the relationships between the demographic variables, general ability and verbal learning test performance. (Master's thesis, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan). Retrieved from http://thesis.lib.cgu.edu.tw/cgi-bin/cdrfb3/gsweb.cgi?o=dstdcdr Logan, G. D. (1978). Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 32-63. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.107.1.32 Ludwig, C., Borella, E., Tettamanti, M., &; De Ribaupierre, A. (2010). Adult age differences in the color Stroop test: A comparison between an item-by-item and a blocked version. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 51, 135-142. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2009.09.040 Macleod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203. Macleod, C. M., &; Macdonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 383-391. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01530-8 MacNiven, J. a. B., Davis, C., Ho, M. Y., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., &; Constantinescu, C. S. (2008). Stroop performance in multiple sclerosis: Information processing, selective attention, or executive functioning? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 805-814. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708080946 Mayr, U., &; Kliegl, R. (1993). Sequential and coordinative complexity: Age-based processing limitations in figural transformations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1297-1320. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.19.6.1297 Moering, R. G., Schinka, J. A., Mortimer, J. A., &; Graves, A. B. (2004). Normative data for elderly african americans for the Stroop color and word test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 61–71. Monahan, J. S. (2001). Coloring single Stroop elements: Reducing automaticity or slowing color processing? Journal of General Psychology, 128, 98-112. doi: 10.1080/00221300109598901 Morrens, M., Hulstijn, W., &; Sabbe, B. (2007). Psychomotor slowing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 1038-1053. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl051 Myerson, J., Hale, S., Zheng, Y., Jenkins, L., &; Widaman, K. F. (2003). The difference engine: A model of diversity in speeded cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin &; Review, 10, 262-288. Parris, B. A., Bate, S., Brown, S. D., &; Hodgson, T. L. (2012). Facilitating goal-oriented behaviour in the Stroop task: When executive control is influenced by automatic processing PLoS ONE, 7, e46994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046994 Parris, B. A., Sharma, D., &; Weekes, B. (2007). An optimal viewing position effect in the Stroop task when only one letter is the color carrier. Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie), 54, 273-280. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.273 Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Barch, D. M., &; Baird, J. W. (1998). The Stroop task and attention deficits in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of card and single-trial Stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology, 12, 414-425. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.414 Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rabbitt, P., &; Goward, L. (1994). Age, information processing speed, and intelligence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 47A, 741-760. Raven, J., Raven, J. C., &; Court, J. H. (1992). Raven's Progressive Matrices-Standard Edition. Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press. Regard, M. (1981). Cognitive rigidity and flexibility: A neuropsychological study. (Ph.D. Thesis, University of Victoria, Victoria BC, Canada). Retrieved from http://voyager.library.uvic.ca/vwebv/holdingsInfo?bibId=208755 Roe, W. T., Wilsoncroft, W. E., &; Griffiths, R. S. (1980). Effects of motor and verbal practice on the Stroop task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 647-650. Rognonia, T., Casals-Colla, M., Sánchez-Benavidesa, G., Quintanaa, M., Manerob, R. M., Calvoa, L., . . . Pe˜Na-Casanovab, J. (2013). Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): Norms for Stroop color-word interference and tower of london-drexel university tests. Neurología. , 28, 73-80. Salo, R., Henik, A., &; Robertson, L. C. (2001). Interpreting Stroop interference: An analysis of differences between task versions. Neuropsychology, 15, 462-471. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.4.462 Salthouse, T. A., &; Madden, D. J. (2008). Information processing speed and aging. In J. DeLuca &; J. H. Kalmar (Eds.), Information processing speed in clinical populations. (pp. 221-241). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor &; Francis. Salthouse, T. A., &; Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, 297-306. Shilling, V. M., Chetwynd, A., &; Rabbitt, P. M. A. (2002). Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: An investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 40, 605-619. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00157-9 Steinberg, B. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Smith, G. E., &; Ivnik, R. J. (2005). Mayo’s older americans normative studies: Age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the trail-making test, the stroop test, and mae controlled oral word association test. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, 329-377. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., &; Spreen, O. (2006). A compedium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662. Treisman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychological Review, 76, 282-299. Trenerry, M., Crosson, B., Deboe, J., &; Leber, W. (1989). Stroop neuropsychological screening test manual. Adessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Troyer, A. K., Leach, L., &; Strauss, E. (2006). Aging and response inhibition: Normative data for the Victoria Stroop Test. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 20-35. doi: 10.1080/138255890968187 Vakil, E., Manovich, R., Ramati, E., &; Blachstein, H. (1996). The Stroop color‐word task as a measure of selective attention: Efficiency in the elderly. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 313-325. doi: 10.1080/87565649609540655 Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Ten Tusscher, M. P. M., Metsemakers, J. F. M., Willems, B., &; Jolles, J. (2001). Visual determinants of reduced performance on the Stroop color-word test in normal aging individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 620-627. doi: 10.1076/jcen.23.5.620.1245 Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., &; Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop color-word test: Influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment, 13, 62-79. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283427 Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., &; Jolles, J. (2008). Detecting the significance of changes in performance on the Stroop color-word test, Rey's Verbal Learning Test, and the Letter Digit Substitution Test: The regression-based change approach. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 71-80. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080028 Van Mourik, R., Oosterlaan, J., &; Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The Stroop revisited: A meta-analysis of interference control in AD/HD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 150-165. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00345.x Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., &; Basak, C. (2006). Aging, task complexity, and efficiency modes: The influence of working memory involvement on age differences in response times for verbal and visuospatial tasks. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 254-280. Verhaeghen, P., &; De Meersman, L. (1998). Aging and the Stroop effect: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 13, 120-126. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.120 Verhaeghen, P., Kliegl, R., &; Mayr, U. (1997). Sequential and coordinative complexity in time–accuracy functions for mental arithmetic. Psychology and Aging, 12, 555-564. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.555 Wensing, T., Levasseur-Moreau, J., Sack, A. T., &; Fecteau, S. (2014). Attentional processing in traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder, traumatic brain injury. In F. Sadaka (Ed.), Traumatic Brain Injury. In D. 10.577 (Series Ed.): InTech. Retrieved from http://www.intechopen.com/books/traumatic-brain-injury/attentional-processing-in-traumatic-brain-injury-and-posttraumatic-stress-disorder. doi: DOI: 10.577 West, R. L., &; Alain, C. (2000). Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology, 37, 179-189. Wogar, M. A., Van Den Broek, M. D., Bradshaw, C. M., &; Szabadi, E. (1998). A new performance-curve method for the detection of simulated cognitive impairment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 327-339.
|