跳到主要內容

臺灣博碩士論文加值系統

(216.73.216.19) 您好!臺灣時間:2025/09/04 03:02
字體大小: 字級放大   字級縮小   預設字形  
回查詢結果 :::

詳目顯示

: 
twitterline
研究生:吳婉寧
研究生(外文):Wan Ning Wu
論文名稱:電腦化史楚普叫色作業之效度檢驗-以台灣樣本為例
論文名稱(外文):Validating a Computerized Stroop Test in a Taiwanese Sample
指導教授:何孟洋何孟洋引用關係
指導教授(外文):M. Y. Ho
學位類別:碩士
校院名稱:長庚大學
系所名稱:職能治療學系
學門:醫藥衛生學門
學類:復健醫學學類
論文種類:學術論文
論文出版年:2014
畢業學年度:102
論文頁數:80
中文關鍵詞:史楚普叫色作業處理速度執行功能差異引擎典範
外文關鍵詞:Stroop TestProcessing SpeedExecutive FunctionsDifference Engine Model
相關次數:
  • 被引用被引用:1
  • 點閱點閱:1594
  • 評分評分:
  • 下載下載:191
  • 收藏至我的研究室書目清單書目收藏:0
背景:史楚普叫色作業的傳統版本已被廣泛使用在臨床和研究上,近年來我們發展出一套電腦化版本之史楚普作業以測量處理速度與色字干擾效果。然而目前仍缺乏此測驗的心理計量基礎的檢驗資料,本研究欲改善此一限制,進而提升此電腦化版本之適用性。方法:本研究共招募年齡介於16至80歲之243位志願者(133位女性及110位男性),並進一步分成五個年齡組,以考驗此作業的心理計量基礎。所有參與者均接受閱讀能力、流體智能及電腦化史楚普叫色作業的評估;同時亦有部分參與者接受執行功能與記憶功能的檢測。之後,再以這些測驗的表現進行相關與迴歸分析,考驗反應時間表現與這些
能力之間的關聯。本研究以相關及迴歸分析檢驗參與者之人口學變項(年齡、性別、教育程度、流體智能),以及反應時間的關聯。將不同年齡組的反應時間比最年輕之參照組(16-25歲)的反應時間作圖,以呈現各組在不同刺激複雜度之相對表現的線性關係。結果:電腦化史楚普作業中,各嘗試的反應時間均隨年齡而增加,且男性的反應時間顯著較女性長。然而,教育程度、閱讀能力及流體智能均與作業反應時間有顯著負相關。史楚普干擾效果亦隨年齡增加,隨流體智能增加而減少,但與性別無顯著相關。各年齡組分別對最年輕參照組之反應時間之作圖顯示其斜率隨年齡增加而越陡,截距則隨年齡增加而減少。結論::本研究的結果顯示,此發展中的電腦化史楚普作業具有可接受的心理計量基礎。此外,本研究結果支持差異引擎典範的假設,即如果一項作業與速度相關,相較於處理速度,以其他認知能力或因素預測個體之表現在群體中所佔的排序時便顯得較不重要。
Background:Conventional Stroop Test has been widely used in in
clinical practice and research. Recently, we have developed a
computerized Stroop Test, which concurrently yields measures for
processing speed and word/color interference effects. However, the
validation procedures of its psychometric properties have not yet been
carried out. The study intended to achieve this aim.
Methods:Two-hundred-and-forty-three healthy volunteers (133 females
and 110 males), aged between 16-80 years (grouping into five 10-year
age bands) underwent a battery of neuropsychological assessment,
including a Chinese word-reading test, a fluid intelligence test, and two
versions of the Computerized Stroop test (CST); and parts of participants
also underwent tests for assessing memory and executive functions.
Correlation and regression analyses were performed on those data to
examine the associations between these abilities. The relationships
between age, sex, education, fluid intelligence, and the reaction times
(RTs) on the CST were examined by correlation and regression analyses.
The linear relationship between the RTs and stimulus complexity in the
CST was examined by plotting the RTs of different stratified age groups
against those of the youngest age group (16-25 years).
Results:The RTs of all types of trials on the CST increased with age; and
the RTs of the males were significantly higher than those of the females.
In contrast, years of education, reading performance, and fluid
intelligence were all negatively correlated with the RTs of the CST. The
word/color interference effects also increased with age and decreased
with fluid intelligence, but were not significantly correlated with sex. The
linear function of the mean RTs of each older group and the youngest
group showed that while the steepness of the slope increased with age, the
values of the intercept progressively declined with age.
Conclusions:The results of this study offered some empirical evidence
for the validity of this newly developed computerized Stroop test. In
addition, the data lend further support for the premise proposed by the
difference engine model, which suggests that if a task is speeded, its other
characteristics appear to be much less important in predicting the position
of an individual’s score in standard deviation unit.
指導教授推薦書………………………………………………………
口試委員審定書………………………………………………………
致謝詞…………………………………………………………………-ⅲ-
ABSTRACT…………………………………………………………...-ⅴ-
中文摘要………………………………………………………………-ⅶ-
CONTENTS……………………………………………………….......-ix-
TABLES AND FIGURES……………………………………………..-xi-
Contents
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION - 1 -
1.1. INTRODUCTION OF THE STROOP TEST - 2 -
1.2. INFLUENCE OF DEMOGRAPHIC FACTORS AND GENERAL ABILITY - 5 -
1.3. THEORETICAL ACCOUNTS OF STROOP EFFECT - 8 -
1.4. IMPLICATIONS FOR CLINICAL PRACTICE - 10 -
1.5. PROCESSING SPEED - 13 -
1.6. THE AIMS OF THE STUDY - 16 -
CHAPTER 2. METHODS - 18 -
2.1. PARTICIPANTS - 18 -
2.2. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTS - 20 -
2.3. PROCEDURE - 25 -
2.4. STATISTICAL ANALYSIS - 26 -
CHAPTER 3. RESULTS - 29 -
3.1. DEMOGRAPHIC DATA - 29 -
3.2. RELIABILITY - 31 -
3.3. CONVERGENT AND DISCRIMINANT VALIDITIES. - 34 -
3.5. TESTING PROCESSING SPEED HYPOTHESES - 41 -
CHAPTER 4. DISCUSSION - 45 -
4.1. RELIABILITY - 45 -
4.2. CONVERGENT AND DISCRIMINANT VALIDITY - 46 -
4.3. INFLUENCE OF DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES - 47 -
4.4. THE STROOP EFFECT. - 49 -
4.5. PROCESSING SPEED AS A GENERAL FACTOR - 51 -
4.6. LIMITATIONS - 53 -
CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION - 55 -
REFERENCE - 56 -
Tables and Figures
TABLES.
TABLE 1. - 30 -
TABLE 2. - 31 -
TABLE 3. - 33 -
TABLE 4. - 34 -
TABLE 5. - 35 -
TABLE 8. - 40 -FIGURES.
FIGURE 2. THE MEAN REACTION TIMES OF ALL TYPES OF TRIALS FOR EACH GROUP AS A FUNCTION OF THE MEAN REACTION TIMES OF THE YOUNGEST GROUP. - 42 -
FIGURE 4. THE STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF ALL TRIALS AS A FUNCTION OF THEIR CORRESPONDING MEAN REACTION TIMES IN EACH GROUP. - 44 -




Reference
Academia Sinica. (1993). Word list with accumulated word frequency in news corpus [in Chinese]. Taipei, Taiwan: Association of Computational Linguistics and Chinese Language Processing.
Anastasi, A., &; Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ, US: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Attix, D. K., Story, T. J., Chelune, G. J., Ball, J. D., Stutts, M. L., &; Hart, R. P. (2009). The prediction of change: Normative neuropsychological trajectories. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 21-38.
Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Brinley, J. F. (1965). Cognitive sets, speed and accuracy of performance in the elderly. In A. T. Welford &; J. E. Birren (Eds.), Behavior, aging and the nervous system (pp. 114–149): Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Bryan, J., &; Luszcz, M. A. (2000). Measures of fluency as predictors of incidental memory among older adults. Psychology and Aging, 15, 483-489. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.483
Bugg, J. M., Delosh, E. L., Davalos, D. B., &; Davis, H. P. (2007). Age differences in Stroop interference: Contributions of general slowing and task-specific deficits. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 155-167.
Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67-83. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.1.67
Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information-processing rate. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.
Cerella, J. (1991). Age effects may be global, not local: Comment on Fisk and Rogers (1991). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 215-223. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.2.215
Chen, E. Y. H., Wong, A. W. S., Chen, R. Y. L., &; Au, J. W. Y. (2001). Stroop interference and facilitation effects in first-episode schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 29-44.
Chen, J., Hale, S., &; Myerson, J. (2007). Predicting the size of individual and group differences on speeded cognitive tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin &; Review, 14, 534-541. doi: 10.3758/BF03194103
Chen, Y.-J., Ho, M.-Y., Chen, K.-J., Hsu, C.-F., &; Ryu, S.-J. (2009). Estimation of premorbid general fluid intelligence using traditional Chinese reading performance. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 63, 500-507.
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., &; Mcclelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332-361. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332
Daigneault, S., Braun, C. M., &; Whitaker, H. A. (1992). Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non-perseverative prefrontal measures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 99-114.
Dash, J., &; Dash, A. S. (1982). Cognitive developmental studies of the Stroop phenomena: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Indian Psychologist, 1, 24-33.
Deary, I. J., Der, G., &; Ford, G. (2001). Reaction times and intelligence differences: A population-based cohort study. Intelligence, 29, 389-399. doi: 10.1016/s0160-2896(01)00062-9
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., &; Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., &; Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Demakis, G. J. (2004). Frontal lobe damage and tests of executive processing: A meta-analysis of the Category Test, Stroop test, and Trail-Making Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 441-450. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510149
Denney, D. R., &; Lynch, S. G. (2009). The impact of multiple sclerosis on patients' performance on the Stroop test: Processing speed versus interference. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 451-458. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090730
Denney, D. R., Lynch, S. G., Parmenter, B. A., &; Horne, N. (2004). Cognitive impairment in relapsing and primary progressive multiple sclerosis: Mostly a matter of speed. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 948-956.
Denney, D. R., Sworowski, L. A., &; Lynch, S. G. (2005). Cognitive impairment in three subtypes of multiple sclerosis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 967-981.
Dyer, F. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes. Memory &; Cognition, 1, 106-120. doi: 10.3758/BF03198078
Flanagan, D. P., &; Harrison, P. L. (2005). Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.
Goethals, I., Audenaert, K., Jacobs, F., Lannoo, E., Van De Wiele, C., Ham, H., . . . Dierckx, R. (2004). Cognitive neuroactivation using SPECT and the Stroop colored word test in patients with diffuse brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 21, 1059-1069.
Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop color and word test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoeling.
Graf, P., Uttl, B., &; Tuokko, H. (1995). Color- and picture-word Stroop tests: Performance changes in old age. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 390-415. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405132
Hale, S., &; Jansen, J. (1994). Global processing-time coefficients characterize individual and group differences in cognitive speed. Psychological Science, 5, 384-389. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00290.x
Hale, S., Myerson, J., &; Wagstaff, D. (1987). General slowing of nonverbal information processing: Evidence for a power law. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 131-136. doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.2.131
Hartley, J. T. (1993). Aging and prose memory: Tests of the resource-deficit hypothesis. Psychology and Aging, 8, 538-551. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.4.538
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., &; Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake &; J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227-249). New York: Oxford University Press.
Heaton, R. K., Ryan, L., &; Grant, I. (2009). Demographic influences and use of demographically corrected norms in neuropsychological assessment. In I. Grant &; K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric and neuromedical disorders (3rd ed., pp. 127-155). New York: Oxford University Press.
Houx, P. J., Jolles, J., &; Vreeling, F. W. (1993). Stroop interference: Aging effects assessed with the Stroop color-word test. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 209-224. doi: 10.1080/03610739308253934
Huang, H. S. (2001). Chinese Graded Word Reading Test [in Chinese]. Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological Publishing.
Hunt, E., &; Lansman, M. (1986). Unified model of attention and problem solving. Psychological Review, 93, 446-461. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.446
Kindt, M., Bierman, D., &; Brosschot, J. F. (1996). Stroop versus Stroop: Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard color-word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 653-661. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00133-X
Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 576-588.
Klein, M., Ponds, R. W., Houx, P. J., &; Jolles, J. (1997). Effect of test duration on age-related differences in Stroop interference. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 77-82.
Lansbergen, M. M., Kenemans, J. L., &; Van Engeland, H. (2007). Stroop interference and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 21, 251-262.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., &; Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lin, H. Y. (2013). An investigation the relationships between the demographic variables, general ability and verbal learning test performance. (Master's thesis, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan). Retrieved from http://thesis.lib.cgu.edu.tw/cgi-bin/cdrfb3/gsweb.cgi?o=dstdcdr
Logan, G. D. (1978). Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 32-63. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.107.1.32
Ludwig, C., Borella, E., Tettamanti, M., &; De Ribaupierre, A. (2010). Adult age differences in the color Stroop test: A comparison between an item-by-item and a blocked version. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 51, 135-142. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2009.09.040
Macleod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203.
Macleod, C. M., &; Macdonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 383-391. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01530-8
MacNiven, J. a. B., Davis, C., Ho, M. Y., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., &; Constantinescu, C. S. (2008). Stroop performance in multiple sclerosis: Information processing, selective attention, or executive functioning? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 805-814. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708080946
Mayr, U., &; Kliegl, R. (1993). Sequential and coordinative complexity: Age-based processing limitations in figural transformations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1297-1320. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.19.6.1297
Moering, R. G., Schinka, J. A., Mortimer, J. A., &; Graves, A. B. (2004). Normative data for elderly african americans for the Stroop color and word test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 61–71.
Monahan, J. S. (2001). Coloring single Stroop elements: Reducing automaticity or slowing color processing? Journal of General Psychology, 128, 98-112. doi: 10.1080/00221300109598901
Morrens, M., Hulstijn, W., &; Sabbe, B. (2007). Psychomotor slowing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 1038-1053. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl051
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Zheng, Y., Jenkins, L., &; Widaman, K. F. (2003). The difference engine: A model of diversity in speeded cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin &; Review, 10, 262-288.
Parris, B. A., Bate, S., Brown, S. D., &; Hodgson, T. L. (2012). Facilitating goal-oriented behaviour in the Stroop task: When executive control is influenced by automatic processing PLoS ONE, 7, e46994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046994
Parris, B. A., Sharma, D., &; Weekes, B. (2007). An optimal viewing position effect in the Stroop task when only one letter is the color carrier. Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie), 54, 273-280. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.273
Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Barch, D. M., &; Baird, J. W. (1998). The Stroop task and attention deficits in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of card and single-trial Stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology, 12, 414-425. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.414
Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rabbitt, P., &; Goward, L. (1994). Age, information processing speed, and intelligence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 47A, 741-760.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., &; Court, J. H. (1992). Raven's Progressive Matrices-Standard Edition. Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press.
Regard, M. (1981). Cognitive rigidity and flexibility: A neuropsychological study. (Ph.D. Thesis, University of Victoria, Victoria BC, Canada). Retrieved from http://voyager.library.uvic.ca/vwebv/holdingsInfo?bibId=208755
Roe, W. T., Wilsoncroft, W. E., &; Griffiths, R. S. (1980). Effects of motor and verbal practice on the Stroop task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 647-650.
Rognonia, T., Casals-Colla, M., Sánchez-Benavidesa, G., Quintanaa, M., Manerob, R. M., Calvoa, L., . . . Pe˜Na-Casanovab, J. (2013). Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): Norms for Stroop color-word interference and tower of london-drexel university tests. Neurología. , 28, 73-80.
Salo, R., Henik, A., &; Robertson, L. C. (2001). Interpreting Stroop interference: An analysis of differences between task versions. Neuropsychology, 15, 462-471. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.4.462
Salthouse, T. A., &; Madden, D. J. (2008). Information processing speed and aging. In J. DeLuca &; J. H. Kalmar (Eds.), Information processing speed in clinical populations. (pp. 221-241). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor &; Francis.
Salthouse, T. A., &; Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, 297-306.
Shilling, V. M., Chetwynd, A., &; Rabbitt, P. M. A. (2002). Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: An investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 40, 605-619. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00157-9
Steinberg, B. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Smith, G. E., &; Ivnik, R. J. (2005). Mayo’s older americans normative studies: Age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the trail-making test, the stroop test, and mae controlled oral word association test. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, 329-377.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., &; Spreen, O. (2006). A compedium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.
Treisman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychological Review, 76, 282-299.
Trenerry, M., Crosson, B., Deboe, J., &; Leber, W. (1989). Stroop neuropsychological screening test manual. Adessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Troyer, A. K., Leach, L., &; Strauss, E. (2006). Aging and response inhibition: Normative data for the Victoria Stroop Test. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 20-35. doi: 10.1080/138255890968187
Vakil, E., Manovich, R., Ramati, E., &; Blachstein, H. (1996). The Stroop color‐word task as a measure of selective attention: Efficiency in the elderly. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 313-325. doi: 10.1080/87565649609540655
Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Ten Tusscher, M. P. M., Metsemakers, J. F. M., Willems, B., &; Jolles, J. (2001). Visual determinants of reduced performance on the Stroop color-word test in normal aging individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 620-627. doi: 10.1076/jcen.23.5.620.1245
Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., &; Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop color-word test: Influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment, 13, 62-79. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283427
Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., &; Jolles, J. (2008). Detecting the significance of changes in performance on the Stroop color-word test, Rey's Verbal Learning Test, and the Letter Digit Substitution Test: The regression-based change approach. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 71-80. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080028
Van Mourik, R., Oosterlaan, J., &; Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The Stroop revisited: A meta-analysis of interference control in AD/HD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 150-165. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00345.x
Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., &; Basak, C. (2006). Aging, task complexity, and efficiency modes: The influence of working memory involvement on age differences in response times for verbal and visuospatial tasks. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 254-280.
Verhaeghen, P., &; De Meersman, L. (1998). Aging and the Stroop effect: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 13, 120-126. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.120
Verhaeghen, P., Kliegl, R., &; Mayr, U. (1997). Sequential and coordinative complexity in time–accuracy functions for mental arithmetic. Psychology and Aging, 12, 555-564. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.555
Wensing, T., Levasseur-Moreau, J., Sack, A. T., &; Fecteau, S. (2014). Attentional processing in traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder, traumatic brain injury. In F. Sadaka (Ed.), Traumatic Brain Injury. In D. 10.577 (Series Ed.): InTech. Retrieved from http://www.intechopen.com/books/traumatic-brain-injury/attentional-processing-in-traumatic-brain-injury-and-posttraumatic-stress-disorder. doi: DOI: 10.577
West, R. L., &; Alain, C. (2000). Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology, 37, 179-189.
Wogar, M. A., Van Den Broek, M. D., Bradshaw, C. M., &; Szabadi, E. (1998). A new performance-curve method for the detection of simulated cognitive impairment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 327-339.


連結至畢業學校之論文網頁點我開啟連結
註: 此連結為研究生畢業學校所提供,不一定有電子全文可供下載,若連結有誤,請點選上方之〝勘誤回報〞功能,我們會盡快修正,謝謝!
QRCODE
 
 
 
 
 
                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                               
第一頁 上一頁 下一頁 最後一頁 top