林幸台、吳武典、胡心慈、郭靜姿、蔡崇建、王振德(2016)。托尼非語文智力測驗(TONI-4)。臺北:心理。
沈帆、孔冰蕾(2006)。寶寶也會說故事。臺北:風車。
林寶貴、黃玉枝、黃桂君、宣崇慧(2009)。修訂學齡兒童語言障礙評量表。臺北:國立台灣師範大學特殊教育中心。
黃瑞珍、簡欣瑜、朱麗璇、盧璐(2011)。華語兒童理解與表達詞彙測驗(第二版)。臺北:心理。
楊立勤(2013)。台灣兒童複雜句的理解與表達之研究(未出版之博士論文)。臺北:國立師範大學。楊坤堂、張世慧、李永源(2006)。學前幼兒與國小低年級兒童口語語法能力診斷測驗。臺北:台北市立師範學院特殊教育中心。
賴曉嬋、王秋鈴(2017)。有效鑑別國小二年級語言障礙兒童和典型發展兒童的敘事指標。特殊教育研究學刊,42(3),33-61。
簡馨瑩、連啟舜(2014)。教師手勢對幼兒故事詞彙理解影響之研究. 教育科學研究期刊,59(2),61。
Arndt, K. B., & Schuele, C. M. (2013). Multiclausal utterances aren't just for big kids: A framework for analysis of complex syntax production in spoken language of preschool-and early school-age children. Topics in Language Disorders, 33(2), 125-139.
Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., & Greenhalgh, T. (2016). CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study: Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS One, 11(7), e0158753.
Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., Catalise‐2 Consortium, Adams, C., & Boyle, C. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068-1080.
Blewitt, P., & Langan, R. (2016). Learning words during shared book reading: The role of extratextual talk designed to increase child engagement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 404-410.
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2004). Gesture development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 173–86
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1468–1480.
Cassell, J., & McNeill, D. (1991). Gesture and the poetics of prose. Poetics Today, 12(3), 375-404.
Chui, K. (2008). Complementary gestures and information types. Language and Linguistics, 9(1), 1-22.
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non‐word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 1-40.
Colmar, S. H. (2014). A parent-based book-reading intervention for disadvantaged children with language difficulties. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 30(1), 79-90.
Contemori, C., & Garraffa, M. (2010). Comparison of modalities in SLI syntax: A study on the comprehension and production of non-canonical sentences. Lingua, 120(8), 1940-1955.
Crowe, L. K., Norris, J. A., & Hoffman, P. R. (2004). Training caregivers to facilitate communicative participation of preschool children with language impairment during storybook reading. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37(2), 177-196.
Cuevas, P., Steines, M., He, Y., Nagels, A., Culham, J., & Straube, B. (2019). The facilitative effect of gestures on the neural processing of semantic complexity in a continuous narrative. NeuroImage, 195, 38-47
Curran, M., & Owen Van Horne, A. (2019). Use of recast intervention to teach causal adverbials to young children with developmental language disorder within a science curriculum: a single case design study. American journal of speech-language pathology, 28(2), 430-447
Demir, Ö. E., Fisher, J. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2014). Narrative processing in typically developing children and children with early unilateral brain injury: Seeing gesture matters. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 815-828.
Diehl, S. F., & Vaughn, B. (2010). Clinical discourse and engagement during shared storybook reading in preschool groups. Seminars in Speech and Language, 31(2), 111-121.
Diessel, H., & Hetterle, K. (2011). Causal clauses: A cross-linguistic investigation of their structure, meaning, and use. In Peter Siemund (Ed.), Linguistic universals and language variation (pp. 21–52). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Donaldson, M. L., Reid, J., & Murray, C. (2007). Causal sentence production in children with language impairments. International journal of language & communication disorders, 42(2), 155-186
Driskell, J. E., & Radtke, P. H. (2003). The effect of gesture on speech production and comprehension. Human Factors, 45(3), 445-454.
Evans J, Saffran J, & Robe-Torres K.(2009) Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 321–36.
Fey M, Catts H, Proctor-Williams K, Tomblin B, Zhang X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 1301–1318.
Fey M, & Proctor-Williams, K. (2000). Imitation, modeling,and recasting in grammar intervention for children with SLI. In D.V.M.
Bishop, & L.B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and Language Impairments:From Theory to Practice (pp. 177-194). East Sussex: Psychology Press.
Fletcher, P., Leonard, L. B., Stokes, S. F., & Wong, A. M. Y. (2005). The expression of aspect in Cantonese-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 621–634.
Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.). (2003). Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S. C., Hedges, L. V., Huttenlocher, J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Small, S. L. (2014). New evidence about language and cognitive development based on a longitudinal study: Hypotheses for intervention. American Psychologist, 69(6), 588-599.
Göksun, T., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2010). How do preschoolers express cause in gesture and speech? Cognitive development, 25(1), 56-68.
Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal behavior, 24(2), 81-103.
Hill, E. L. (2001). Non‐specific nature of specific language impairment: a review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International journal of language & communication disorders, 36(2), 149-171.
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 137(2), 297-315.
Hostetter, A. B., Bieda, K., Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Knuth, E. J. (2006). Don’t just tell them, show them! Teachers can intentionally alter their instructional gestures. Proceedings of the 28th Annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1523–28). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Isbell, R., Sobol, J., Lindauer, L., & Lowrance, A. (2004). The effects of storytelling and story reading on the oral language complexity and story comprehension of young children. Early childhood education journal, 32(3), 157-163.
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological science, 16(5), 367-371.
Jenkins, T., Coppola, M., & Coelho, C. (2017). Effects of gesture restriction on quality of narrative production. Gesture, 16(3), 416-431.
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological review, 99(1), 122-149.
Keenan, J. & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Understanding the relation between comprehension and production. In H. W. Dechert. & M. Raupach (Eds.).Psycholinguistic models of production (pp. 149-155). Norwook, N.J.: ABLEX.
Kress, G. (2003) Literacy in the New Media Age. London: Taylor & Francis.
Lavelli, M. A. N. U. E. L. A., Barachetti, C., & Florit, E. (2015). Gesture and speech during shared book reading with preschoolers with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1191-1218.
Lavelli, M., Barachetti, C., Majorano, M., Florit, E., Brotto, C., & Miottello, P. (2019). Impacts of a shared book‐reading intervention for Italian‐speaking children with developmental language disorder. International journal of language & communication disorders, 54(4), 565-579.
Lavelli, M., & Majorano, M. (2016). Spontaneous gesture production and lexical abilities in children with specific language impairment in a naming task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(4), 784-796.
Lely, H. K. V. D., & Harris, M. (1990). Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired children. Journal of speech and hearing disorders, 55(1), 101-117.
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Leonard, L., Bishop, D. V. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2000). Speech and language impairments in children. Causes, Characteristics, Intervention. Oxford: Psychology Press.
Lum, J. A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48(9), 1138-1154.
Lwin, S. M. (2016). It's story time! Exploring the potential of multimodality in oral storytelling to support children's vocabulary learning. Literacy, 50(2), 72-82.
Macoun, A., & Sweller, N. (2016). Listening and watching: The effects of observing gesture on preschoolers’ narrative comprehension. Cognitive Development, 40, 68-81.
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project [Child Language Data Exchange System]. Web.
Mainela‐Arnold, E., Alibali, M. W., Hostetter, A. B., & Evans, J. L. (2014). Gesture–speech integration in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 761-770.
Marinellie, S. A. (2004). Complex syntax used by school-age children with specific language impairment (SLI) in child–adult conversation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37(6), 517-533.
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you?. New York: Dial Press.
Mayer, M. (1975). One frog too many. New York: Dial Press.
Mayer, M. (1978). A boy, a dog and a frog. New York: Dial Press.
McGregor, K. K., Rohlfing, K. J., Bean, A., & Marschner, E. (2009). Gesture as a support for word learning: The case of under. Journal of child language, 36(4), 807-828.
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago press.
McNeil, N. M., Alibali, M. W., & Evans, J. L. (2000). The role of gesture in children's comprehension of spoken language: Now they need it, now they don't.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 131-150.
Miller, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Finneran, D. (2008). Grammaticality judgements in adolescents with and without language impairment. International journal of language & communication disorders, 43(3), 346-360.
Montgomery, J. W., & Evans, J. L. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 269–288.
Nicoladis, E., Marentette, P., & Navarro, S. (2016). Gesture frequency linked primarily to story length in 4–10-year old children’s stories. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 45(2), 189-204
Noble, C. H., Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Lieven, E. (2018). Keeping it simple: the grammatical properties of shared book reading. Journal of Child Language, 45(3), 753-766.
Noble, C., Sala, G., Peter, M., Lingwood, J., Rowland, C., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. (2019). The impact of shared book reading on children’s language skills: a meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 28, Article 100290.
Norbury, C. F., Gooch, D., Wray, C., Baird, G., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., & Pickles, A. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 57(11), 1247-1257.
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96(3), B101-B113
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and cognitive processes, 24(2), 190-217.
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2000). Tense and temporality: A comparison between children learning a second language and children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 834-847.
Rice, M. L. (2013). Language growth and genetics of specific language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(3), 223-233.
Riches, N. G., Tomasello, M., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Verb learning in children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1397–1411.
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain and language, 88(2), 229-247.
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental science, 12(1), 182-187.
Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development. First language, 28(2), 182-199.
Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human development, 18(1-2), 65-79.
Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-age children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 324-339.
Singleton, N. C., & Saks, J. (2015). Co-speech gesture input as a support for language learning in children with and without early language delay. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 22(2), 61-71.
Steele, S. C., & Mills, M. T. (2011). Vocabulary intervention for school-age children with language impairment: A review of evidence and good practice. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 27(3), 354-370.
Stites, L. J., & Özçalışkan, Ş. (2017). Who did what to whom? Children track story referents first in gesture. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 46(4), 1019-1032.
Theakston, A. L., Coates, A., & Holler, J. (2014). Handling agents and patients: Representational cospeech gestures help children comprehend complex syntactic constructions. Developmental psychology, 50(7), 1973-1984.
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 40(6), 1245-1260.
Tsang, K. K., & Stokes, S. F. (2001). Syntactic awareness of Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 28(3), 703-739.
Vallotton, C. D. (2009). Do infants influence their quality of care? Infants’ communicative gestures predict caregivers’ responsiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(4), 351-365.
Vogt, S. S., & Kauschke, C. (2017). With some help from others' hands: Iconic gesture helps semantic learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 3213-3225.
Wagner, C. R., Nettelbladt, U., Sahlén, B., & Nilholm, C. (2000). Conversation versus narration in pre‐school children with language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(1), 83-93.
Whitehurst, G. J., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Payne, A. C., Crone, D. A., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention in Head Start. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(4), 542-555.
Wray, C., Saunders, N., McGuire, R., Cousins, G., & Norbury, C. F. (2017). Gesture production in language impairment: it's quality, not quantity, that matters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(4), 969-982.
Yaden, D. B. (2003). Parent-child storybook reading as a complex adaptive system: Or ‘‘An igloo is a house for bears’’. In A. van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, & E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children, parents and teachers (pp. 336–362). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1993). Can developmentally delayed children's language development be enhanced through prelinguistic intervention?. In A. Kaiser & D. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing children’s communication: Research foundations for intervention (pp. 35–62). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Zwitserlood, R., Wijnen, F., van Weerdenburg, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). ‘MetaTaal’: enhancing complex syntax in children with specific language impairment—a metalinguistic and multimodal approach. International journal of language & communication disorders, 50(3), 273-297.